Prime Sound

Pros and Cons: Listening to Music While Studying for Optimal Focus

Alecia Steen

Integrating music into your study routine may seem like a mere method of enjoyment, but it transcends this perception. It is, in fact, a powerful cognitive tool that has been proven to have remarkable effects on our learning capabilities. Research continues to shed light on the profound influence of music on studying, unveiling an array of benefits that contribute to more effective, focused, and enjoyable learning experiences.

Advantages of Studying With Music

1. an impressive aid in studying.

Venturing into the depths of how music helps us study, it’s essential to recall Dr. Gordon Shaw’s groundbreaking theory, ‘ The Mozart Effect ‘. This theory originated in the 1990s when Shaw extensively studied brain theory and spatial reasoning in problem-solving. His research with students led to the intriguing conclusion that frequent exposure to Mozart’s “ Sonata for Two Pianos in D Major ” boosted their IQ by nine points.

Not only does music provide a pleasant background for studying, but it also enhances endurance, keeping students engaged for extended periods. The persistence needed to master new material often clashes with the monotony of the task. Music comes into play here, transforming the tedious process into an engaging, even enjoyable experience.

2. A Powerful Catalyst for Focus

Music plays a crucial role in maintaining focus during study sessions in the era of endless distractions. It acts as a gentle, steady undercurrent, tuning out external disturbances. As you immerse yourself in your study material, music provides a consistent, comforting backdrop, allowing your mind to anchor itself firmly to the task.

Music mitigates the whirl of distracting thoughts and allows your brain to steer its attention to studying. It’s akin to a cognitive anchor, helping your brain bypass the temptations of wandering thoughts and bringing them back to the study material.

3. An Incredible Instrument for Concentration

Scientific studies, including brain imaging scans, have highlighted the effect of music on concentration. Listening to music activates the brain’s left and right hemispheres simultaneously, significantly bolstering learning abilities. By triggering different areas of your brain, music helps maintain its agility and health. Thus, nurturing your ability to concentrate by merely tuning into some serene sounds is within your grasp.

The influence of music on concentration is multifaceted. It helps reduce anxiety and facilitates healthy emotional processing, leaving your brain free to concentrate on the task at hand.

4. A Potent Enhancer of Academic Performance

Incorporating music into learning curriculums has repeatedly proven beneficial, significantly improving academic performance. For example, a primary school in Bradford achieved a remarkable rise in SATS results by merely integrating more music into the curriculum.

5. A Dynamic Motivator for Studying

One of the most profound challenges in studying is sustaining the motivation to persist through complex material. Music, with its varied tones, rhythms, and melodies, injects an element of enjoyment into the learning process, rekindling motivation and interest.

Creating a personalized learning playlist can serve as a backdrop to your study routine. Listening to your favorite tracks can inspire focus and stimulate your eagerness to learn, thus making studying an enjoyable endeavor rather than a dreaded task.

6. A Proven Memory Booster

Music is a well-recognized mnemonic device. It triggers the memory centers in the brain, making recall more manageable. To put it simply, music can play a vital role in helping you remember what you’ve studied. The melody and rhythm of music can link to specific information, making it easier to retrieve when needed.

Songs with catchy lyrics often get stuck in our heads. This phenomenon, often referred to as an “ earworm ,” can be put to productive use in learning. Pairing important information with melodies can help you remember details with greater accuracy.

7. A Mindful Way to Manage Stress

The soothing power of music is no secret. Numerous studies show that music can significantly reduce stress and anxiety levels. When you’re feeling overwhelmed by your study load, listening to relaxing music can help restore calm, allowing you to refocus and study more effectively.

Music has a unique link to our emotions; thus, it can serve as an extremely effective stress management tool. It can be a powerful medium to connect with our feelings, helping us process them more healthily and ultimately enhancing our ability to learn.

8. A Creative Pathway for Problem Solving

Disadvantages of Music During Study Sessions

While the benefits of integrating music into study sessions are indeed substantial, it’s crucial also to be aware of its potential drawbacks. Just as music can enhance cognitive function, it may hinder optimal learning under certain circumstances. Understanding these potential pitfalls is essential to leverage music’s benefits while minimizing its potential for distraction or inefficiency.

1. A Potential Distraction

While music can promote focus, it can also have the opposite effect, particularly when it contains lyrics. Lyrics can interfere with the processing of linguistic information, such as reading or writing. In essence, your brain may struggle to focus on the study material because it’s also attempting to process the words in the song. This is especially true if the music’s language aligns with your study material.

Moreover, complex musical compositions with intricate harmonies and melodies can similarly draw attention away from the task at hand. Rather than serving as a gentle backdrop, such music can command cognitive resources, leading to divided attention.

2. An Interrupter of Deep Learning

Research suggests that silence is sometimes more beneficial for complex tasks that require deep cognitive processing. Some learners may find music interrupting their thought process, making solving complex problems or grasping challenging concepts more difficult. Deep learning requires an undisturbed mental space, and music can fragment this continuity for some individuals.

3. A Misleading Sense of Mastery

Listening to music while studying can create a more enjoyable learning environment, which can sometimes lead to an inflated sense of understanding. Research indicates that students who study with music tend to believe they’ve learned the material better than they actually have. This can be problematic when it comes to recalling and applying information during an examination or in a practical setting.

4. An Unwanted Emotional Influence

While music can help manage stress and enhance mood, it can also evoke strong emotions that might distract from studying. For example, a song that reminds you of a particular event or person might trigger a flood of memories and emotions, leading your mind away from the study material.

5. A Potential Cause of Overstimulation

Listening to music while studying can lead to sensory overload, particularly for individuals with certain learning styles or neurological conditions. For instance, individuals with ADHD may find music overly stimulating and distracting. Similarly, individuals with auditory processing issues or those who are particularly sensitive to sound might find that music more hindrance than a help.

While music can indeed be a powerful tool to enhance studying, it is not universally beneficial. Understanding one’s learning style and preferences is vital to ensure the best use of music during study sessions. Just as the right kind of music under the right conditions can boost learning, the wrong type, or using it inappropriately, can have the opposite effect. Thus, balance and self-awareness are key when leveraging music in the pursuit of knowledge.

Does listening to music improve GPA?

No direct scientific evidence suggests that listening to music while studying will necessarily improve your GPA. The relationship between music and studying is complex and depends on various factors, such as the type of music, the task at hand, and individual learning styles. For instance, some students may find that certain types of instrumental or classical music improve their concentration and subsequently enhance their study effectiveness. However, the potential impact on GPA would also depend on many other factors related to study habits, comprehension, test-taking skills, and so forth.

Is it better to listen to music while working or to work in silence?

The choice between working with music or in silence is largely personal and can depend on the task at hand. Silence may be the best option if the work involves complex cognitive processing or linguistic comprehension, as it allows for deeper concentration. On the other hand, for more mundane or repetitive tasks, music can make the process more enjoyable and may help maintain focus. Importantly, music without lyrics or with a consistent rhythm tends to be less distracting. The key is understanding your work style and the nature of the task.

Why does music help me focus with ADHD?

Research has suggested that individuals with ADHD can benefit from listening to music during tasks that require concentration. Music, particularly with a steady rhythm, can stimulate the brain’s production of certain chemicals like dopamine and norepinephrine, which play crucial roles in attention and focus. Moreover, listening to music can make the task more enjoyable, which may improve motivation and persistence in individuals with ADHD. However, this isn’t universally true for everyone with ADHD. The type of music, the task, and personal preferences all play a role. Trying different approaches and seeing what works best for you is important.

Final Thoughts

The role of music in study and work environments is complex and multifaceted. The effects it has on productivity, focus, and creativity are contingent on a multitude of factors, including the nature of the task, the type of music, and the individual’s personal preferences and learning style. While research provides some guidance, the final decision on whether to incorporate music into study or work routines rests upon trial and error, as individuals gauge what methods best optimize their performance and well-being.

Furthermore, it’s essential to consider the limitations and potential drawbacks of this practice. Music can enhance mood and focus and can also be a source of distraction, particularly when engaging in complex cognitive tasks or when the music includes discernable lyrics. Hence, finding the right balance is crucial. Music is a tool that, when used strategically, can potentially improve both productivity and enjoyment in work or study environments. It underscores the importance of personalizing our learning or working styles, aligning our habits to our unique preferences, and continuously experimenting with ways to optimize our performance.

post

I got on this website fr a school prject and i loed it, thankyall!

should students listen to music while doing homework

That’s the same reason I’m here! xD

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Curious Kids: is it OK to listen to music while studying?

should students listen to music while doing homework

Lecturer in Psychology, University of Wollongong

Disclosure statement

Timothy Byron does not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and has disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

University of Wollongong provides funding as a member of The Conversation AU.

View all partners

should students listen to music while doing homework

I am in year 11 and I like to listen to music when I am studying, but my dad says that my brain is spending only half of its time studying and the other half is distracted by listening. He says it is better to leave my phone out of my room and concentrate on studying rather than listening to music. Is it OK to listen to songs when I am studying? – Robert, Year 11 student.

should students listen to music while doing homework

It’s a good question! In a nutshell, music puts us in a better mood, which makes us better at studying – but it also distracts us, which makes us worse at studying.

So if you want to study effectively with music, you want to reduce how distracting music can be, and increase the level to which the music keeps you in a good mood.

Read more: Curious Kids: Why do adults think video games are bad?

Music can put us in a better mood

You may have heard of the Mozart effect – the idea that listening to Mozart makes you “smarter”. This is based on research that found listening to complex classical music like Mozart improved test scores, which the researcher argued was based on the music’s ability to stimulate parts of our minds that play a role in mathematical ability.

However, further research conclusively debunked the Mozart effect theory: it wasn’t really anything to do with maths, it was really just that music puts us in a better mood.

Research conducted in the 1990s found a “Blur Effect” – where kids who listened to the BritPop band Blur seemed to do better on tests. In fact, researchers found that the Blur effect was bigger than the Mozart effect, simply because kids enjoyed pop music like Blur more than classical music.

Being in a better mood likely means that we try that little bit harder and are willing to stick with challenging tasks.

should students listen to music while doing homework

Music can distract us

On the other hand, music can be a distraction – under certain circumstances.

When you study, you’re using your “working memory” – that means you are holding and manipulating several bits of information in your head at once.

The research is fairly clear that when there’s music in the background, and especially music with vocals, our working memory gets worse .

Likely as a result, reading comprehension decreases when people listen to music with lyrics . Music also appears to be more distracting for people who are introverts than for people who are extroverts, perhaps because introverts are more easily overstimulated.

Some clever work by an Australia-based researcher called Bill Thompson and his colleagues aimed to figure out the relative effect of these two competing factors - mood and distraction.

They had participants do a fairly demanding comprehension task, and listen to classical music that was either slow or fast, and which was either soft or loud.

They found the only time there was any real decrease in performance was when people were listening to music that was both fast and loud (that is, at about the speed of Shake It Off by Taylor Swift, at about the volume of a vacuum cleaner).

But while that caused a decrease in performance, it wasn’t actually that big a decrease. And other similar research also failed to find large differences.

should students listen to music while doing homework

So… can I listen to music while studying or not?

To sum up: research suggest it’s probably fine to listen to music while you’re studying - with some caveats.

It’s better if:

  • it puts you in a good mood
  • it’s not too fast or too loud
  • it’s less wordy (and hip-hop, where the words are rapped rather than sung, is likely to be even more distracting)
  • you’re not too introverted.

Happy listening and good luck in your exams!

Read more: Why do old people hate new music?

Hello, curious kids! Have you got a question you’d like an expert to answer? Ask an adult to send your question to [email protected]

  • Music therapy
  • Curious Kids
  • effective studying

should students listen to music while doing homework

Head of IT Operations

should students listen to music while doing homework

Research Fellow, Agricultural Sustainability

should students listen to music while doing homework

ARDC Project Management Office Manager

should students listen to music while doing homework

Lecturer / Senior Lecturer in Indigenous Knowledges

should students listen to music while doing homework

Professor in Physiotherapy

Texas A and M University College of Liberal Arts Logo

Does Listening to Music Really Help You Study?

Experts from the department of psychology explain whether or not music is a helpful study habit to use for midterms, finals, and other exams.

graohic of listening to music

By Mia Mercer ‘23

Picture of girl studying with headphones

Students have adopted several studying techniques to prepare for exams. Listening to music is one of them. However, listening to music may be more distracting than helpful for effective studying.

There’s no season quite like an exam season on a university campus. Students turn to varying vices to help improve their chance of getting a good grade. While some chug caffeine, others turn up the music as they hit the books.

Although listening to music can make studying more enjoyable, psychologists from the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences have found that this popular study habit is more distracting than beneficial. 

“ Multitasking is a fallacy; human beings are not capable of truly multitasking because attention is a limited resource, and you can only focus on so much without a cost,” cognitive psychologist Brian Anderson said. “So when you’re doing two things at the same time, like studying and listening to music, and one of the things requires cognitive effort, there will be a cost to how much information you can retain doing both activities.” 

In basic terms of memory, Anderson explained that we do a better job of recalling information in the same conditions in which we learn the material. So when studying for an exam, it’s best to mimic the exam conditions. 

“If you have music going on in the background when you study, it’s going to be easier to recall that information if you also have music on in the background when you take the exam,” Anderson said. “However wearing headphones will almost certainly be a violation during most exams, so listening to music when you’re studying will make it harder to replicate that context when you’re taking an exam.” 

Even though experts suggest listening to music can hinder your ability to retain information while studying, some students choose to continue the practice. Steven Smith, cognitive neuroscientist for the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences , provided some suggestions for students who wish to continue this study habit. 

 “In general, words are distracting,” Smith shared. “So if you want to listen to music while you study, try to listen to something that does not have words, or if it does have words, hopefully, it’ll be in a language that you don’t understand at all, otherwise that’s going to distract from the stuff you’re trying to study.”

Smith also suggested listening to familiar background music, because it’s less distracting than something new or exciting. Additionally, Smith provided some principles that generally result in better exam results. 

“Make sure your studying is meaningful because comprehension gets you so much further than raw repetition,” Smith shared. “Also, you must test yourself, because it’s the only way you can learn the material; this is called the testing-effect. And finally, try to apply the spacing-effect, where you spread out your study sessions rather than cramming your studying all together, allowing for better memory of the material.”

Regardless of how students decide to study for exams, it’s important to remember that we all learn differently.

“There are individual differences between everyone,” Smith said. “Some people need a study place that is boring, predictable, and exactly the same so that they can concentrate, and others find it more beneficial to go to different places to study. It’s true that there are different personalities, so try and find what study habit works best for you.” 

  • The College at Work
  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences
  • Preparing for Exams

Does Listening to Music While Doing Homework Affect Your Grade in School?

Music is a powerful art form that can bring up emotions, inspire motivation and alter your mood. Students frequently listen to music while studying to make the process less painful and, in some cases, because they believe music will help them learn. The effects of listening to music while studying are mixed, however, and depend upon the type of music you listen to as well as the degree to which it distracts you.

should students listen to music while doing homework

Music With Lyrics

Music with lyrics activates the language-processing centers of the brain, and the University of Phoenix advises that this can be distracting. Particularly if you're reading or studying subjects within the humanities, the act of processing musical lyrics as you try to process the words you're studying can make studying more challenging. Students who listen to music with lyrics may have more difficulty concentrating and may struggle more to recall the information they've learned.

Advertisement

Article continues below this ad

Instrumental Music

Robin Harwood, et al. point to the "Mozart Effect" in their textbook "Child Psychology." The "Mozart Effect" is the belief that listening to classical music can improve intelligence; it is based upon a single study that was subsequently refuted. Instrumental and classical music won't make you smarter, according to Harwood, et al. But this music can have a relaxing, soothing effect and is less distracting than music with lyrics.

More For You

Factors that affect listening comprehension, how to get smarter in math, what are the advantages & disadvantages of the literature-based approach to teaching reading, the difference between a reading disability & dyslexia, interactive skills for reading, staying focused.

A 2005 study published in "Psychology of Music" found that workers who listened to music while working had higher productivity than those who didn't. The study's authors speculate that this could be because music boosts mood, improving motivation. Particularly among students who are struggling to remain motivated to complete their work, music might provide a respite from the stress and exhaustion of studying and inspire them to keep at it.

Context-Dependent Learning

People recall information more effectively when they're doing so in the same environment in which they initially learned it, according to the textbook "Educational Psychology." Students who listen to music while studying will be better at recalling the information they've learned if they also listen to music during tests -- an opportunity most students don't have. This might mean that listening to music can make recalling information more challenging, particularly for students who transition from listening to loud music to taking a test in a silent classroom.

  • Psychology of Music: The Effect of Music Listening on Work Performance
  • Mind the Science Gap: Does Music Help You Study?
  • University of Phoenix: Should You Listen to Music While Studying?
  • USA Today College: Should You Listen to Music While You Study?
  • Child Psychology; Robin Harwood et al.; 2008
  • Educational Psychology; Anita Woolfolk; 2006

Van Thompson is an attorney and writer. A former martial arts instructor, he holds bachelor's degrees in music and computer science from Westchester University, and a juris doctor from Georgia State University. He is the recipient of numerous writing awards, including a 2009 CALI Legal Writing Award.

March 3, 2020

Does Music Boost Your Cognitive Performance?

The answer depends on your personality

By Cindi May

should students listen to music while doing homework

Getty Images

Music makes life better in so many ways. It elevates mood , reduces stress and eases pain . Music is heart-healthy , because it can lower blood pressure , reduce heart rate and decrease stress hormones in the blood. It also connects us with others and enhances social bonds . Music can even improve workout endurance and increase our enjoyment of challenging activities .

The fact that music can make a difficult task more tolerable may be why students often choose to listen to it while doing their homework or studying for exams. But is listening to music the smart choice for students who want to optimize their learning?

A new study by Manuel Gonzalez of Baruch College and John Aiello of Rutgers University suggests that for some students, listening to music is indeed a wise strategy, but for others, it is not. The effect of music on cognitive functioning appears not to be “one-size-fits-all” but to instead depend, in part, on your personality—specifically, on your need for external stimulation. People with a high requirement for such stimulation tend to get bored easily and to seek out external input. Those individuals often do worse , paradoxically, when listening to music while engaging in a mental task. People with a low need for external stimulation, on the other hand, tend to improve their mental performance with music.

On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing . By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.

But other factors play a role as well. Gonzalez and Aiello took a fairly sophisticated approach to understanding the influence of music on intellectual performance, assessing not only listener personality but also manipulating the difficulty of the task and the complexity of the music. Whether students experience a perk or a penalty from music depends on the interplay of the personality of the learner, the mental task, and the music.

In the study, participants first completed the Boredom Proneness Scale , which is a personality test used to determine need for external stimulation. They then engaged in an easy cognitive task (searching for the letter A in lists of words) and a more challenging one (remembering word pairs). To control for practice and fatigue effects, half of the subjects completed the easy task first, while the other half completed the challenging one first. Participants finished both tasks under one of three sound conditions: (a) no music, (b) simple music or (c) complex music. All of the music was instrumental, and music complexity was manipulated by varying the number of instruments involved in the piece. Simple music included piano, strings and synthesizer, while complex music added drums and bass to the simple piece.

The data suggest that your decision to turn music on (or off) while studying should depend on your personality. For those with a high need of external stimulation, listening to music while learning is not wise, especially if the task is hard and/or the music is complex. On the simple task of finding A’s, such subjects’ scores for the music condition were the same (for simple music) or significantly worse (for complex music) than those for the silent condition. On the complex task of learning word pairs, their performance was worse whenever music was played, regardless of whether it was simple or complex.

For those with a low need of external stimulation, however, listening to music is generally the optimal choice. On the simple task of findings A’s, such participants’ scores for the music condition were the same (for simple music) or dramatically better (for complex music) than those for the silent condition. On the complex task of learning word pairs, the participants showed a small but reliable benefit with both simple and complex music, relative to silence.

The results suggest that there are substantial individual differences in the impact of music on cognitive function, and thus recommendations regarding its presence in the classroom, study hall or work environment may need to be personalized. Students who are easily bored and who seek out stimulation should be wary of adding music to the mix, especially complex music that may capture attention and consume critical cognitive resources that are needed for successful task completion. On the other hand, students with a low need for stimulation may benefit significantly from the presence of music, especially when completing simple, mundane tasks.

Before students decide to slip in their earbuds, though, they should carefully consider both their musical selection and the nature of the task. All of the music used in the present study was instrumental, and lyrical music will likely be more complex. Complexity appears to increase arousal, and the Yerkes-Dodson law suggests that a moderate level of arousal produces optimal performance. When there is too little or too much arousal, performance drops. Thus, the benefits of music for those with a low need for external stimulation that were observed here could diminish or even disappear with the added complexity of lyrics.

Similarly, increases in the complexity of a cognitive task might also reduce or eliminate the benefit of music. Although the “complex” task used in this study (learning word pairs) was only moderately challenging, the increase in complexity, relative to the simple task, was enough to reduce music’s positive effect. With a highly challenging cognitive task (e.g., text comprehension or exam preparation), even those with a low need for external stimulation may fail to show such an effect with music.

With the right (low-need-for-stimulation) personality, the right (instrumental) music and the right (low-to-moderately-difficult) task, the presence of music may significantly improve cognitive functioning. Given the many other physical, emotional and psychological benefits of music, that subscription to Spotify just might pay for itself!

Cindi May is a professor of psychology at the College of Charleston. She explores avenues for improving cognitive function and outcomes in college students, older adults and individuals who are neurodiverse.

SA Mind Vol 31 Issue 3

Journal Name Logo

Journal of Cognition

Ubiquity Press Logo

  • Download PDF (US English) XML (US English)
  • Alt. Display

Research Article

Should we turn off the music music with lyrics interferes with cognitive tasks.

  • Alessandra S. Souza
  • Luís Carlos Leal Barbosa

People often listen to music while doing cognitive tasks. Yet, whether music harms or helps performance is still debated. Here, we assessed the objective and subjective effects of music with and without lyrics on four cognitive tasks. College students completed tasks of verbal and visual memory, reading comprehension, and arithmetic under three conditions: silence, instrumental music, and music with lyrics. Participants judged their learning during and after each condition. Music with lyrics hindered verbal memory, visual memory, and reading comprehension ( d ≈ –0.3), whereas its negative effect ( d = –.19) on arithmetic was not credible. Instrumental music (hip-hop lo-fi) did not credibly hinder or improve performance. Participants were aware of the detrimental impact of the lyrics. Instrumental music was, however, sometimes perceived as beneficial. Our results corroborate the general distracting effect of background music. However, faulty metacognition about music’s interfering effect cannot fully explain why students often listen to music while studying.

  • auditory distraction
  • judgments of learning
  • metacognition

We listen to music, intentionally or not, while performing different activities ( Rentfrow, 2012 ). In most tasks, music is a foreign stimulus, as when background music is playing while we are reading, studying, or solving mathematical problems. So far, both benefits and costs of background music on these tasks have been observed. Yet, people seem to have low insight into music’s potential impairing effects, often reporting actively choosing music as a background to study. We present further evidence that music with lyrics is generally detrimental to cognitive performance, while instrumental music has a more minor, not credible effect. People were usually aware of the distracting effect of the lyrics, yet they tended to believe that instrumental music was beneficial. Metacognition was, therefore, not wholly faulty, and may only partially explain inefficient study habits.

Background Music: Why is the Evidence Mixed?

In their meta-analysis, Kämpfe et al. ( 2011 ) reported that the effect of background music on cognition was overall null because positive and negative results averaged each other out (see also De la Mora Velasco & Hirumi, 2020 ). There are several candidate variables to explain this variability. One potential variable is the presence of lyrics. A recent systematic review did not find a relation between the type of music and whether positive or negative effects were observed ( de la Mora Velasco & Hirumi, 2020 ). Yet, a meta-analysis focused on reading observed a larger detrimental impact of music with lyrics than instrumental music ( Vasilev et al., 2018 ). To further clarify this matter, we assessed the effects of both music with lyrics and instrumental music on cognitive performance.

Another potential moderating variable is the type of learning task. The most commonly used task is verbal memory, with the majority of studies finding negative effects in this task ( Cassidy & MacDonald, 2007 ; Furnham & Bradley, 1997 ; Groot & Smedinga, 2014 ; Nittono, 1997 ; Reaves et al., 2016 ; Smith & Morris, 1977 ; Su & Wang, 2010 ). However, there are also reports of positive ( de Groot, 2006 ) and neutral effects on this task ( Jäncke et al., 2014 ; Jäncke & Sandmann, 2010 ; Küssner et al., 2016 ; Linek et al., 2011 ; Nguyen & Grahn, 2017 ). The use of visual tasks is rare, with only one study reporting worse visual memory when listening to music with lyrics compared to instrumental music ( Belsham & Harman, 1977 ). Results for reading comprehension tasks are also mixed. Yet, in a recent meta-analysis, Vasilev et al. ( 2018 ) estimated an overall small but credible impairment of background music on reading (Hedges’s g = –0.19). Finally, fewer studies tested the effect of music on arithmetic tasks observing benefits ( Miller & Schyb, 1989 ; Proverbio et al., 2018 ; Wolf & Weiner, 1972 ), no change ( Chew et al., 2016 ; Manthei & Kelly, 1999 ; Mowsesian & Heyer, 1973 ; Wolfe, 1983 ), and even costs ( Christopher & Shelton, 2017 ; Tucker & Bushman, 1991 ). The present study considered these multiple task domains to gain a clearer picture of the impact of background music on cognition.

Variability could also be due to sampling. Studies usually use between-subject designs with overall low sample sizes (mean N = 67), which afford less power to detect effects ( de la Mora Velasco & Hirumi, 2020 ; Kämpfe et al., 2011 ). To more firmly establish the polarity and size of the effects of background music, we employed a within-subjects design with a relatively large sample.

Despite the mixed findings regarding the impact of background music on learning, students often report listening to music while studying or doing coursework. This raises the question regarding the subjective effect of music, or in other words, how people perceive the impact of music on their own performance.

Metacognition and the Effects of Background Music

One recent survey estimated that people listen to music ca. 40% of the time while reading or writing, and 20% of the time while memorizing ( Goltz & Sadakata, 2021 ). In other surveys, these values were of 60% while studying and 20% while reading ( David et al., 2015 ; Kiss & Linnell, 2022 ). In general, participants were divided in assessing the impact of background music as costly or beneficial, but people that study with music tended to perceived it as beneficial ( Goltz & Sadakata, 2021 ).

Few studies considered the subjective impact of music; but the general finding is that people have poor metacognition on this subject. Hallam and Godwin ( 2015 ) assessed the impact of calming vs . exciting music (as opposed to silence) on the quality of story-writing in children. Exciting music was detrimental to performance, yet this type of music was perceived as more enjoyable and hence as beneficial. Anderson and Fuller ( 2010 ) assessed reading comprehension in 7 th and 8 th graders in a musical and silence condition. Performance was lower in the musical condition, and this effect was larger for students that reported a preference for listening to music while studying. Christopher and Shelton ( 2017 ) asked participants to judge their performance in musical and silence conditions while performing reading and arithmetic tasks. Background music hindered performance in both tasks, but participants were unaware of its detrimental effect.

Overall, this scarce literature points to a metacognitive blind-spot: students seem to enjoy studying while listening to music, and hence they fail to perceive its true impact on performance. To offer more data on this subject, we asked participants to provide subjective assessments during and after completing our tasks.

Present Study

Our goals were three-fold. First, we aimed to determine the effect of background music on four different cognitive domains: verbal and visual memory, reading comprehension, and arithmetic. Second, we contrasted performance in an instrumental music and music with lyrics condition to silence, using a within-subjects design and a relatively large sample of participants ( N = 113–123). The inclusion of these two types of music permitted us to address the role of music type, while our sample-size provided a power of 80% to detect effects as low as d = .26. Third, we collected metacognitive judgments during and after the completions of our tasks to identify if people misperceive the impact of background music on their performance.

We formulated the following hypotheses. First, given that music with lyrics contains speech information, we expected this condition to create the largest interference in tasks that involve verbal processing. This prediction is based on the Irrelevant Speech Effect , namely the impairment of performance observed when irrelevant background speech is presented concurrently with a memory task ( Colle & Welsh, 1976 ; LeCompte et al., 1997 ; Salamé & Baddeley, 1982 , 2013 ). Our second prediction was that instrumental music would be less disruptive than music with lyrics. In the memory literature, background sounds were also found to disrupt performance ( Jones & Macken, 1993 ). This prediction is however not without controversy. Instrumental music could also be predicted to produce better performance due to changes in emotional states, e.g., by relaxing participants ( Kiss & Linnell, 2022 ).

Finally, we predicted that participants would show low metacognitive insight about the impact of background music on their performance. Given the scarce data on the literature, we had no specific prediction regarding an interaction with task or time-point in which the judgments were made (i.e., during or after the task).

Participants

Psychology students from the University of Porto participated in this online study in exchange for extra-course credits. We choose to collect data online since it permitted the recruitment of a large sample of participants with reasonable data-quality ( Uittenhove et al., 2023 ). The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Porto (approval number 2022/02–05). Participants completed an Informed Consent Form online and were debriefed regarding the study purposes at the end.

The study was divided in two online sessions lasting 30 min each. We aimed to collect data of at least N = 100. Actual sample-size was determined based on participation sign-up. We accepted all submissions made during the spring semester of 2022 (from April to June). A total of 136 students participated, yet due to desistance or data-loss, we only obtained data-sets of both sessions of N = 100. Yet, to use as much data as possible, we considered all data we had for each of the tasks in isolation. Session 1 was completed by 123 students ( n = 57 completed the verbal/math tasks; n = 66 completed the visual/reading tasks). Session 2 was completed by 113 students ( n = 56 completed the verbal/math tasks; n = 57 completed the visual/reading tasks). Only 105 participants filled the demographics questionnaire at the end of Session 2. Respondents were aged between 18 and 58 years ( M = 20.74, SD = 5.5), with 5% identifying themselves as “male”, 90.5% as “female”, 4% as “non-binary/third gender” and 1% as “rather not say”.

Experimental Design

All variables were manipulated in a within-subjects design. Participants completed four tasks, with each task being completed three times in blocks that varied in terms of the background sound. In the Silence block, participants completed the task under normal ambient noise. Since the study was collected online, this reflected the usual ambient noise of the participants. We instructed participants to be in a quiet place, and to remove all possible distractions from the environment (TV, music, cell-phones, social media, animals, or other people). In the Instrumental Music block, participants listened to instrumental music while completing the task. We selected a genre of instrumental music known as lo-fi hip-hop due to its popularity among students for use while studying ( Winston & Saywood, 2019 ). In the Lyrical Music block, participants listened to popular music with European Portuguese lyrics.

Before the start of the tasks in each session, participants were told to fetch a head-set or to be in an ambient were they could leave the computer sound enabled. Then they were instructed to adjust the volume to a comfortable level while listing to a sample sound. They were instructed to not change the volume level while working on the tasks. Before each task block, they were warned about the upcoming condition (silence, instrumental music, or music with lyrics). The order of the silence, instrumental, and lyrical blocks within each task was randomly determined for each participant and task.

Participants completed four tasks: a verbal recall task, a visual recall task, a reading comprehension, and an arithmetic problem-solving task.

In the verbal recall task , participants learned three lists with 20 words each. One random word-list was assigned to be learned in each experimental condition. Words were drawn from the Minho word pool ( Soares et al., 2017 ), which is a data-basis with 3,800 European-Portuguese words, ranked on imageability, concreteness and subjective frequency. Each of the three lists contained five words with high frequency ( M = 189.7 per million; SD = 87.8) and high concreteness ( M = 6.4; SD = 0.3), five words with high frequency ( M = 289.5; SD = 233.3) and low concreteness ( M = 2.7; SD = 0.3), five words with low frequency ( M = 0.2; SD = 0.2) and high concreteness ( M = 6.3; SD = 0.3), and five words with low frequency ( M = 0.6; SD = 0.2) and low concreteness ( M = 2.8; SD = 0.2). Additionally, four words were used as practice words. The word lists used are available in the Online Supplementary Materials.

For the visual recall task , three lists of 20 images were created, and one random list was assigned to be learned in each experimental condition. The images were selected from the ones used by Sutterer and Awh ( 2016 ). The solid color of each image was randomly sampled from 360 continuous colors selected from a color wheel defined in the CIELAB color space with the following parameters: L = 70, a = 20, b = 38, with a radius of 60 ( Zhang & Luck, 2008 ). The color of each image remained the same for all participants. The images used and their colors are presented in the Online Supplementary Materials.

For the reading comprehension task , six lists of 20 sentences were constructed. Two lists were allocated to be processed in each experimental condition. These lists were made by adapting the Reading Test-Sentence Comprehension [Teste de Leitura: Compreensão de Sentenças (TELCS)], which is a Portuguese adaptation of the Lobrot’s Lecture 3 (L3) reading test ( de Araújo Vilhena et al., 2016 ). Since the TELCS only contained 36 sentences, 84 additional sentences were created to obtain a total of 120 sentences. The TELCS is composed of a list of 36 incomplete sentences, where the last word is missing, for example, the sentence “ Foi difícil ter uma boa nota naquele ” [“It was hard to have a good grade on that”]. Participants are asked to select one word out of five available options to correctly complete the sentence. In the example above, the correct option was “ exame ” [“exam”]. The five options share at least one common characteristic. They can be visually similar by sharing a similar orthography and having letters in common with one another (e.g., “ enxada ”). They can be phonologically similar by sharing the same last phoneme and thus rhyming (e.g., “ enxame ”). Or they can be semantically proximal by having close meanings to each other (e.g., “ estudo ”). Using this model for the generation of the distractor words, the 84 sentences and their respective options were created. There was also an effort to make sure that the number of letters in the sentences did not differ much from list to list (the number of letters was between 880 and 980). The original sentences from the TELCS were distributed equally between the six lists and in order (e.g., sentence 1 in list 1, sentence 2 in list 2, etc.). The newly generated sentences were distributed equally between the lists. The sentence lists are presented in the Online Supplementary Materials.

For the arithmetics task , six lists consisting of 20 problems were constructed. Two lists were randomly assigned to be completed in each experimental condition. Each list contained six problems that followed the model “a × b + c” (e.g., 4 × 9 + 8 = ?), six problems that followed the model “a × b–c” (e.g., 7 × 5–3 = ?) and eight problems that followed the model “a + b × c” (e.g., 9 + 3 × 8 = ?). The list also contained five response options for each problem: the correct answer and four incorrect answers, which were generated by randomly assigning four numbers which were between the two closest multiples of 10 to the correct answer. For example, if the correct answer was 17, then the randomly generated wrong answers were between 10 and 20, for instance 14, 19, 11, 20. The operation lists are presented in Online Supplementary Materials.

Each task was completed in three separate blocks, each representing a different experimental condition, with music being presented concurrently in two of them. Therefore, a total of eight songs were chosen to be presented across the four tasks. Four of the eight songs contained only lo-fi instrumental music. They were retrieved from the Youtube channel “Lofi Girl” which had 10,2 million subscribers and 1 165 531 540 overall views on 23/03/2022. Its most viewed video had 75 688 049 views and it consists of a compilation of 28 songs. Using a random number generator, four numbers were randomly chosen from 1 to 28 (3, 25, 17 and 2). The songs corresponding to each of these positions were chosen, which were: “Cotton Cloud” by Fatb; “Gyoza” by less.people; “Alone Time” by Purrple Cat; “Snowman” by WYS. The remaining four songs were popular songs with lyrics in European Portuguese. Using the website Acharts.co, we consulted which were the most popular songs in Portugal on 20/02/2022, and then chose the four most popular ones. The first four songs which followed our criterion were at the spots 13, 14, 26, and 28. The songs were: “Onde Vais” by Bárbara Bandeira e Carminho; “Mais ou Menos Isto” by Rita Rocha; “Fato treino do City” by Sippinpurpp; “Como Se Te Fosse Perder” by Anselmo Ralph e Diogo Piçarra. The pairing of each task and song was the same for all participants.

All tasks were designed to be completed online. Tasks were programmed using the free and open source lab.js online experimenter builder ( Henninger et al., 2020 , 2022 ). This builder uses HTML and Java-script as the programming language. The code to run all tasks is available at: https://osf.io/xcv6e/ .

The experiment was divided into two sessions lasting ca. 30 min each, which were completed in separate days. On one session, participants completed the verbal memory recall and the arithmetic tasks. On the other session, they completed the visual recall and the reading comprehension tasks. This was done to assure minimal interference between the tasks. The order of tasks within each session was randomly determined for each participant. We counterbalanced the order of the sessions. Half of the participants completed the verbal recall and arithmetic tasks on the first session, and the visual recall and reading comprehension tasks in the second session. For the remaining ones, the order was reversed.

In the first session, participants were presented an informed consent describing the study. Participants were asked to consent to the terms of the experiment before advancing to the task. Next, participants created an individual code to connect the data of the two online sessions. Then, participants were guided to complete a volume test so that they could adjust the volume on their computer to a comfortable level before proceeding. They were advised to not change the volume throughout the whole experiment. Participants were then instructed on the completion of each of the two tasks to be carried out in that session. At the end of the first session, they received a certificate of completion of the task, and were instructed to contact the experimenter to receive the link to the second session.

The sequence of events in Session 2 was similar with three major differences: (1) it did not contain the informed consent form; (2) at the end of the session, participants completed demographic questions (age, gender, schooling levels of the participant and the participant’s parents) and questions about their study habits (i.e., their average daily study hours, how frequently they listen to music while studying and how frequently they study in noisy places); and (3) it contained a debriefing about the experiment at the end.

All four tasks began with an introductory instruction and an example trial. In the word recall task , each task block consisted of the presentation of a sequence of 20 words in the center of the screen, one-by-one, for memorization. As shown in Figure 1A , before each word, a fixation point appeared in the middle of the screen for 1.5 s, followed by a blank screen for 0.3 s. Afterwards, the word was presented for 1.7 s. In the music blocks, music was presented only while learning the memoranda. After all words were displayed, participants were asked about the number of words they will be able to recall (aka a judgement of learning). They answered by moving a slider ranging from 0 to 20. Then the recall phase began: participants were instructed to recall as many of the memorized words as they could in any order. They were shown a grid with 20 cells. Every time they entered a word followed by enter, the cursor moved to the next cell. When they were finished recalling the words, they clicked on a “finish” button at the bottom of the page to move on. They could recall between 0 and 20 words, with no time restriction. Once they entered a word they could not edit their response. After finishing the recall, participants rated the difficulties they felt in the memorization and in the recall part by using two sliders that ranged from 0= “Little difficulty” to 10 = “A lot of difficulty”.

Figure 1 Illustration of the Flow of Events in Word and Visual Recall Tasks.

Illustration of the Flow of Events in Word and Visual Recall Tasks.

Note : Each task was completed three times, once in silence, once while listening to instrumental music (lo-fi) and once with music with lyrics. Background music was presented during the study phase only.

For each block, one of the three word-lists created for this task was randomly used (see Materials section). After the completion of all blocks in this task, participants completed a series of follow-up questions regarding the manipulation of background music. First, they were asked to rate how much the two types of music affected their performance compared to performance on the silence block. They answered using two sliders (one for the instrumental and one for the music with lyrics condition) that ranged from –10 (“Very negatively”) to 10 (“Very positively”). Next, participants were asked to provide some information in relation to the songs: if they knew the songs; how much they enjoyed the instrumental music from 0 to 10; and the music with lyrics from 0 to 10.

In each block of the visual recall task , a sequence of 20 colored images were displayed on the center of the screen one-by-one. As shown in Figure 1B , before each image, a fixation cross appeared on the middle of the screen for 1 s, followed by a blank screen for 0.5 s. Afterwards, the colored image was presented for 3 s. After all images were displayed, the participant was asked to make a judgment of learning, similarly to the verbal recall task . Then the recall phase began, in which all 20 images were probed in random order. Each image was presented first in gray color surrounded by a gray wheel. When the participant hovered the cursor over the grey wheel, the color of the image changed continuously. This is because the grey wheel was covering a continuous color wheel. By moving the mouse around the grey wheel, the participant continuously adjusted the color of the probed image. Participants were instructed to click with the mouse when they thought they had selected the correct color. There was no time-limit to respond in the recall phase. Thereafter, the next to-be-recalled image was presented. Between images, a fixation point appeared for 1 s. When they were finished recalling all images, the next block started. After the completion of all blocks in this task, participants completed the same follow-up questions regarding the manipulation of music as described for the verbal recall task.

Each block of the reading comprehension task was divided into two parts, separated by a judgement of learning. Each part consisted of the presentation of a sequence of 20 sentences (i.e., one of the lists generated for this task). As illustrated in Figure 2A , each sentence was preceded by a fixation point (1.5 s), followed by a blank screen (0.3 s). Afterwards, a sentence appeared on the center of the screen for 2 s. A blank screen appeared once more for 0.3 s before the five options were displayed for 4 s. The five options were randomly contained inside rectangles, all centered and positioned vertically in the center of the screen. Participants had to click on the option they thought completed the sentence correctly. If they did not respond within 4 s, a time-out was registered and the program moved to the next event. Time-outs were counted as incorrect answers. After the first 20 sentences, participants were asked to make a judgment of learning by predicting how many sentences they would complete correctly in the next half of the block. They answered by moving a slider ranging from 0 to 20. Then, the next 20 sentences were presented. It followed the same structure as the first one. In blocks in which background music was played, the music was continuously looping while participants completed both block parts and the judgment of learning rating. For each participant, the six sentence-lists created for this task were randomly distributed across the silence, instrumental and lyrical blocks, and the two task parts therein. After the completion of all blocks in this task, participants completed the same follow-up questions regarding the manipulation of background music as detailed previously.

Figure 2 Illustration of the Flow of Events in the Reading Comprehension and the Arithmetic Tasks.

Illustration of the Flow of Events in the Reading Comprehension and the Arithmetic Tasks.

Note : Each task was completed three times, once in silence, once while listening to instrumental music (lo-fi) and once with music with lyrics. Background music was presented during completion of the task and the judment of learning rating.

The blocks of the arithmetic task followed a similar structure to the ones in the reading comprehension task . Each block was divided into two parts, each consisting of the presentation of a sequence of 20 problems (see Figure 2B ). Each problem was anticipated by a fixation point (1.5 s), followed by a blank screen (0.3 s). Afterwards, a problem and the respective response options appeared simultaneously for 8 s. For each participant, the five options were randomly displayed inside five rectangles, all centered and positioned along the horizontal axis in the middle of the screen. Participants clicked on the option they thought was the correct answer to the problem. If they did not respond within 8 s, a time-out was registered and the programed moved to the next event. After the first 20 problems, participants were asked to make a judgment of learning by predicting how many problems they believed they would solve correctly in the next block half. They answered by moving a slider ranging from 0 to 20. Next, the final sequence of 20 problems followed. In blocks with background music, the music was presented continuously through the presentation of the problems and the judgment of learning. For each participant, the six problem-lists created for this task were randomly distributed across the three condition blocks (i.e., silence, instrumental, and lyrical), and block half. After the completion of all blocks, participants answered the follow-up questions regarding the manipulation of music.

Data Analysis

The data was processed and analyzed using R ( R Core Team, 2021 ) and Rstudio ( Rstudio Team, 2020 ). Statistical inferences were performed using the BayesFactor package ( Morey & Rouder, 2015 ) using the default prior settings. We used Bayesian Inference to assess the evidence for the presence vs. absence for the effect of background music on performance. In essence, Bayesian inference provides a comparison of the likelihood of the data in light of the alternative hypothesis (i.e., there is an effect of a manipulation) and the null hypothesis (i.e., no effect). The ratio of the likelihood of these hypotheses is the Bayes Factor (BF). Here we report BF 10 , which represents the strength of the evidence for the alternative hypothesis over the null. BFs should be interpreted as a continuous measure. BF 10 > 1 provides evidence for the alternative hypothesis, and BF 10 < 1 provides evidence for the null hypothesis. For example, a BF 10 = 10 indicates that the data is 10 times more likely under the alternative hypothesis than the null. Conversely, a BF 10 = 0.10 indicates that the data is 10 times more likely under the null than the alternative hypothesis.

The dependent variables in our study varied depending on the task. The following objective measures of performance were considered. For the verbal recall task, we computed the proportion of correctly recalled words over the maximum number of words learned (i.e., 20). For the visual recall task, first, we computed a measure of recall error ( Souza et al., 2014 ). Recall error reflects the absolute distance on the wheel between the angle of the correct response and the angle of the response given by the participant. For example, if the correct color was at the angle 30°, and the participant recalled the color at the angle 57°, the recall error was of 27° in that trial. The measure of recall error ranges from 0° (perfect recall) to 180° (recall of the color at the opposite location on the wheel). An average performance close to 90° is consistent with guessing. To increase comparability with the remaining measures, we rescaled the recall error variable to range between 0 and 1 (as in proportion correct), with larger values reflecting better performance. We applied the following equation: (180-recallError)/180. In this rescaled measure, 0.5 indicates guessing, and 1.0, perfect recall. For the reading comprehension and arithmetic tasks, we computed two measures: the proportion of correct responses (with time-outs being considered as wrong responses), and the time to respond correctly in the task (in seconds) since the onset of the response options. The maximum response time in the reading and arithmetic tasks was 4 and 8 s, respectively.

We collected two subjective measures. Judgments of learning were requested in all four tasks and reflected the predictions of performance during the task. The predictions were made in absolute values (e.g., how many words do you think you will remember? ) and they were transformed in proportions by dividing it by the maximum value (i.e., 20 in all tasks). Finally, participants also made post-task ratings of how much they believed the two types of musical background affected their performance in comparison to the silence condition (from very negatively = –10 to very positively = 10).

Research Transparency and Openness

All task materials, data, and analysis scripts are available in the page of the project at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/xcv6e/ ( Souza & Barbosa, 2023 ).

Outlier Detection

We first screened the data for potential outliers. For the reading comprehension and arithmetic tasks, we calculated the 99 th quantile of a binomial distribution with 40 trials (i.e., the number of trials per condition in each task) and with a probability of success of 0.2 on each trial (one correct option out of five), and divided this by the total number of trials. This cutoff value (0.35) indicates the level of performance that is no better than chance. Then, we excluded participants with an overall level (i.e., across all conditions) of correct responses smaller or equal than this cutoff. No outliers were found in the reading comprehension task ( N = 123), but seven were excluded in the arithmetic task (final N = 106). For the visual recall task, a similar method was used, but using an average recall error of 80° as the cutoff point instead. In this task, random responding is assumed to generate responses close to 90°. Five outliers were removed (final N = 118). In the verbal recall task, we simply calculated the proportion of correct recalled words, and since this task does not have a guessing level, we included all respondents ( N = 113).

Table 1 presents the evidence for the main effect of background music in the objective performance measures (i.e., proportion correct and time to respond) and in the subjective measures of performance (i.e., judgments of learning and post-task ratings) in all four tasks.

Evidence (Bayes Factor, BF) for the Main Effect of Background Music (One-Way ANOVA), and for the Pairwise Contrast of Conditions (t-tests). For the Condition Comparisons, the Effect-Size (Cohen’s d) and its 95% Confidence Interval is Also Provided.

Note . Positive values of d reflect better performance of the musical condition stated first; negative values reflect worse performance in this condition.

Verbal Recall

Figure 3 presents the mean proportion of correct answers (Panel A), judgements of learning (Panel B), and post-task ratings (Panel C) in the verbal recall task. Background music credibly affected proportion correct (see Table 1 ), this being mainly due to a small decrease in recall accuracy ( d = –.32) in the lyrical condition compared to silence. The instrumental condition produced a smaller ( d = –.16) decrement, which made this condition not credibly different from either the silence or lyrical conditions. Judgments of learning showed a similar pattern, with participants predicting lower performance in the lyrical than in the remaining two conditions, which did not differ. At the end of the task, participants accurately evaluated instrumental music as having no credible effect compared to silence (value close to 0), but lyrical music as leading to a cost (value < 0).

Figure 3 Results of the Verbal Recall task. Panel A. Proportion of correct responses in each condition: silence, mean = 0.42; instrumental music, mean = 0.40; music with lyrics, mean = 0.37; Panel B. Judgments of learning in each condition: silence, mean = 0.42; instrumental music, mean = 0.41; music with lyrics, mean = 0.34; Panel C. Post-task rating of the effect of music compared to silence: instrumental music, mean = -.07; music with lyrics, mean = -2.92.

Results of the Verbal Recall Task. Panel A. Proportion of Correct Answers. Panel B. Judgments of Learning. Panel C. Post-Task Ratings.

Note : Instr. = instrumental music. Individual data is shown as a small overlaid cloud of dots (slightly jittered along the x-axis for better visibility). The sample mean is presented as a large dot. Error bars are the 95% within-subject confidence interval ( Morey, 2008 ).

Visual Recall

Figure 4 shows the proportion of correct responses (Panel A), judgments of learning (Panel B), and post-task ratings (Panel C) in the visual recall task. There was strong evidence for an effect of background music in visual recall (see Table 1 ). This was mainly due to a small decrease ( d = –.33) in recall accuracy in the lyrical condition compared to silence. The effect of instrumental music was also negative ( d = –.23), but it was ambiguous. Subjective measures also indicated that participants were aware of the detrimental impact of music with lyrics on their performance – both when they rated learning during the task as well as in the post-task ratings. In contrast, participants predicted similar performance in the instrumental condition as in the silence condition when asked during the task (i.e., judgments of learning, Panel B), but not in the post-task evaluations (Panel C) in which they considered instrumental music as beneficial to performance (values > 0).

Figure 4 Results of the Visual Recall task. Panel A. Proportion of correct responses in each condition: silence, mean = 0.80; instrumental music, mean = 0.78; music with lyrics, mean = 0.77; Panel B. Judgments of learning in each condition: silence, mean = 0.36; instrumental music, mean = 0.33; music with lyrics, mean = 0.30; Panel C. Post-task rating of the effect of music compared to silence: instrumental music, mean = 1.28; music with lyrics, mean = -1.71.

Results of the Visual Recall Task. Panel A . Proportion of Correct Answers. Panel B . Judgments of Learning. Panel C . Post-Task Rating.

Reading Comprehension

Figure 5 presents the mean proportion of correct answers (Panel A), the average time to respond correctly (Panel B), judgments of learning (Panel C), and the post-task rating (Panel D) in the reading comprehension task. Although performance was generally very high in this task, there was substantial evidence for a music effect on the proportion of correct responses (see Table 1 ). This was due to somewhat higher accuracy in the instrumental ( M = 0.95, SD = 0.22) compared to the lyrical condition ( M = 0.93, SD = 0.26). It is worth noting that the contrast between the lyrical and silence conditions produced a small performance decrement ( d = –.19) of the same size as reported in a recent meta-analysis ( Vasilev et al., 2018 ). Another unique aspect is that this is the only task in which instrumental music tended to improve performance, but note that this effect was not credible. There was no effect on the time to respond correctly in the task.

Figure 5 Results Reading Comprehension task. Panel A. Proportion of correct responses in each condition: silence, mean = 0.94; instrumental music, mean = 0.95; music with lyrics, mean = 0.93; Panel B. Average time to respond in each condition: silence, mean = 1.78 s; instrumental music = 1.79 s; music with lyrics = 1.80 s; Panel C. Judgments of learning in each condition: silence, mean = 0.82; instrumental music, mean = 0.79; music with lyrics, mean = 0.74; Panel D. Post-task rating of the effect of music compared to silence: instrumental music, mean = 2.31; music with lyrics, mean = -1.36.

Results of the Reading Comprehension Task. Panel A . Proportion of Correct Answers. Panel B . Average Time to Respond Correctly. Panel C . Judgments of Learning. Panel D . Post-Task Ratings.

Note : Instr. = instrumental music. Individual data is shown as a small overlaid cloud of dots (slightly jittered along the x-axis for better visibility). The sample mean is presented as a large dot. Error bars are the 95% within-subject confidence intervals ( Morey, 2008 ).

With regard to the subjective measures of performance, during the completion of the task, judgments of learning ( Figure 5C ) were not credibly different between the instrumental and silence conditions, but were worse in the lyrical condition. In the post-task ratings ( Figure 5D ), however, participants judged instrumental music as beneficial (values > 0), whereas lyrical music was perceived as detrimental (values < 0) compared to silence.

Arithmetic Task

Figure 6 shows the proportion of correct responses (Panel A), time to respond correctly (Panel B), judgments of learning (Panel C), and post-task rating (Panel D) in the arithmetic task. There was strong evidence against an effect of background music in objective measures of performance (i.e., proportion correct and time to respond; see Table 1 ). Yet, when considering the proportion correct measure, the pattern was the same as in the previous tasks, with a slightly larger impairment for lyrical ( d = –.11) than instrumental music ( d = –.05) compared to silence. In Judgments of learning ( Figure 6C ), there was ambiguous evidence for an effect, mainly due to somewhat lower predictions in the lyrical condition compared to silence. In the post-task ratings ( Figure 6D ), participants accurately predicted that instrumental music was inconsequential to performance, but overestimated the detrimental impact of lyrical music.

Figure 6 Results of the Arithmetic task. Panel A. Proportion of correct responses in each condition: silence, mean = 0.69; instrumental music, mean = 0.68; music with lyrics, mean = 0.67; Panel B. Average time to respond in each condition: silence, mean = 4.79 s; instrumental music = 4.80 s; music with lyrics = 4.77 s; Panel C. Judgments of learning in each condition: silence, mean = 0.51; instrumental music, mean = 0.48; music with lyrics, mean = 0.46; Panel D. Post-task rating of the effect of music compared to silence: instrumental music, mean = 0.35; music with lyrics, mean = -2.87.

Results of the Arithmetic Task. Panel A. Proportion of Correct Answers. Panel B. Average Time to Respond Correctly. Panel C. Judgments of Learning. Panel D. Post-Task Ratings.

Music Preferences and Study Habits

After completing all three blocks of each task, participants were asked whether they knew each song, and how much they liked each song on a scale ranging from 0 (“ I didn’t like it ”) to 10 (“ I liked it a lot ”). For the verbal recall task, both songs were equally liked (instrumental: M = 5.50, SD = 2.64; lyrical: M = 5.23, SD = 3.36), BF 10 = 0.13. For the visual recall task, the song with lyrics was liked slightly more (instrumental: M = 5.45, SD = 2.81; lyrical: M = 6.27, SD = 3.2) but this difference was ambiguous, BF 10 = 1.29. For reading comprehension, the instrumental music ( M = 6.04, SD = 2.70) was substantially more liked than the one with lyrics ( M = 3.67, SD = 3.47), BF 10 = 1.6×10 5 . Finally, for the arithmetic task, the instrumental music ( M = 5.78, SD = 2.66) was also more liked than the one with lyrics ( M = 4.18, SD = 3.31), BF 10 = 153.98.

Participants were also asked “ Did you know any of the songs that were played? ”. They could answer by selecting one of four options: “ Yes, both ”, “ No, neither ”, “ Yes, but only the instrumental song ” and “ Yes, but only the song with lyrics ”. Table 2 presents song knowledge by task. In the recall tasks, the majority of participants knew the song with lyrics, whereas in the reading comprehension and arithmetic tasks the majority of participants was not familiar with neither song. Throughout all tasks, the instrumental song (lo-fi) was the least recognized one.

Music Knowledge of Each Song in the Different Tasks.

Regarding study habits, participants reported studying for an average of three hours daily ( M = 3.11, SD = 2.6). When asked how often they study while listening to music, 14.29% said always, 20% said often, 22.86% said sometimes, 24.76% said rarely and 18.1% said never. In a similar question, participants were asked how often they study in a noisy environment, to which 0% replied always, 5.71% replied often, 29.52% replied sometimes, 43.81% replied rarely and 20.95% replied never. This indicates that a noisy environment is a less common context of study for our participants than a musical environment.

We assessed the objective and subjective impact of background music on four cognitive tasks. Verbal and visual memory were significantly worse when these tasks were completed with music with lyrics compared to silence. In the reading comprehension task, participants responded more correctly in the instrumental than in the lyrical condition. Only in the arithmetic task, background music had no credible effect. Whereas music with lyrics was generally detrimental, our instrumental music (lo-fi) did not credibly hinder or improve performance. Subjectively, the music with lyrics was always perceived as impairing, even when it did not credibly hinder performance. Instrumental music, in contrast, was not seen as distracting, and retrospectively, participants tended to assess it as beneficial.

Hypotheses of the Impact of Music on Learning

Based on the Irrelevant Speech and Irrelevant Sound Effects , we hypothesized a graded negative impact of background music: music with lyrics should be the most impairing, whereas instrumental music should lay in-between the lyrical and silence conditions. As shown in Table 1 , our results generally agreed with these predictions. Music with lyrics had a credible, but relatively small effect ( d = ca. 0.3) in three of our tasks. For arithmetic, the negative impact was smaller ( d = –.19) and not credible, however, the direction of the effect was the same as in the remaining tasks. In contrast to silence, instrumental music had a much smaller impact on performance ( d s ranging from –.23 to .14), which could not be credibly determined. There are two possible reasons for this. First, the impact of music with lyrics was already small, and given the even smaller interference produced by instrumental music, there was not much room to measure their difference. Second, our sample-size was not powered to find very small effects. Larger sample-sizes will be required to firmly establish if instrumental music harms performance or whether it is inconsequential.

Our findings agree with previous studies in indicating that background music has a general distracting effect ( de la Mora Velasco & Hirumi, 2020 ; Kämpfe et al., 2011 ; Vasilev et al., 2018 ), and that the size of this distracting effect is moderated by music type. Music with lyrics contains speech, which has privileged access to our cognition. Although our study did not control that the lyrical and instrumental conditions differed only in terms of speech presence, a recent study showed that speech was the determinant variable in generating a distraction effect in a continuous reading task ( Vasilev et al., 2022 ). Our results corroborate this assumption. This speech-related effect may be due to either semantic or phonological interference with the ongoing task ( Vasilev et al., 2022 ).

Our instrumental music (hip-hop lo-fi) had a much milder, and not credible performance effect. This new musical genre is becoming popular among students, being generally advertised as music to listen while studying ( Lo-Fi Study Music , n.d. ; “The Benefits of Studying to Lo-Fi Music,” 2021 ; Winston & Saywood, 2019 ). Although lo-fi is sometimes referred to as a study booster, our findings lend little support for this claim. The only task in which lo-fi tended to improve performance (although not credibly) was reading comprehension. In the remaining tasks, it had the same negative trend as music with lyrics, only smaller. Our results therefore suggest that given the choice between music with lyrics vs. instrumental music to study to, instrumental music should be preferred. Yet, based on performance indicators alone, this condition should not be recommended or preferred over silence.

Differences Between Task Domains

We assessed the impact of background music on a set of four cognitive tasks spanning different domains to assess if task type moderates the effect of background music. We employed standard memory tasks from the literature on verbal and visual memory. For reading comprehension and arithmetic problem solving, we adapted tasks to allow for a more continuous measure of processing through the online recording of accuracy and response times.

We obtained a reasonably consistent pattern across tasks: performance was generally best under silence, intermediate in the instrumental condition, and worse in the lyrical condition. Hence, we have little evidence that task type moderates the cognitive effects of background music. Reading comprehension slightly deviated from this pattern due to the somewhat performance increase in the instrumental condition. This was also the easiest task (accuracy > 90%). We adapted a reading test for online data-collection, enforcing short processing times in attempt to make the task more challenging. Yet, it was still quite easy for our sample of college students. Future studies may consider creating sentences with a more challenging structure to reduce ceiling effects.

In general, we expected lyrical music to have a more pronounced negative effect on verbal tasks. Yet, the general trend was the same for all tasks. Although it may seem surprising that visual memory was similarly impacted as verbal memory by music with lyrics, recent evidence is mounting that visuospatial tasks tend to be impaired by verbal as well as non-verbal means ( Morey, 2018 ). Furthermore, presentation times in our visual task were sufficiently long to allow for participants to try to verbaly label the memoranda, and previous research from our lab has shown that labeling can improve episodic long-term memory ( Overkott & Souza, 2022 ). Music with lyrics may therefore interfere with this labeling process, thereby harming performance.

Awareness of the Impact of Music

Our second main aim was to assess awareness of the effect of music. Based on the previous literature ( Christopher & Shelton, 2017 ; Hallam & Godwin, 2015 ), we hypothesized that metacognition would be low. Surprisingly, participants were quite aware of the negative impact of music with lyrics on performance. For all tasks, music with lyrics was perceived as distracting, even when it did not credibly hinder performance (arithmetic task). On the converse, instrumental music was perceived as not distracting. Perhaps the contrasting effect to the music with lyrics was so large that after completing the tasks, participants even assessed instrumental music as beneficial.

Our study is one of the first to chart subjective perceptions of the impact of music during and after cognitive tasks, ad to show that metacognition about the interfering impact of music was not faulty. Yet, the puzzle remains regarding why students often report listening to music while studying. Future studies may need to include not only subjective assessments of performance success (as done here) but also emotional and motivational effects of music listening. It is possible that cognitive, motivational, and emotional variables drive subjective experiences with music that may conjointly guide the self-regulation of study habits.

Data Accessibility Statement

Materials, data, and analysis code can be accessed at: https://osf.io/xcv6e/ .

Ethics and Consent

The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Porto (approval number 2022/02–05). Participants completed a written online Informed Consent Form before the start of Session 1 and were debriefed regarding the study purposes at the end of Session 2.

Acknowledgements

This article is based on the data of the master thesis submitted by the second author under the supervision of the first author to the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Porto. We thank Dr. Nuno Gaspar for his valuable contribution as a committee member.

Funding Information

A. S. Souza was supported by a grant (CEECINST/00159/2018) awarded to the Center for Psychology of the University of Porto (FCT/UIDB/00050/2020) by Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (FCT, Portugal).

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Author Contributions

AS supervised the project. AS and LB conceptualized the study, programmed the experimental tasks, and analyzed the data. AS curated the data, performed the final analysis and wrote the manuscript – original draft. LB provided comments. Both authors approved the final manuscript for submission.

Anderson, S. A., & Fuller, G. B. (2010). Effect of music on reading comprehension of junior high school students. School Psychology Quarterly , 25, 178–187. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021213  

Belsham, R. L., & Harman, D. W. (1977). Effect of Vocal vs Non-Vocal Music on Visual Recall. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 44(3), 857–858. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1977.44.3.857  

Cassidy, G., & MacDonald, R. A. R. (2007). The effect of background music and background noise on the task performance of introverts and extraverts. Psychology of Music , 35(3), 517–537. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607076444  

Chew, A. S.-Q., Yu, Y.-T., Chua, S.-W., & Gan, S. K.-E. (2016). The effects of familiarity and language of background music on working memory and language tasks in Singapore. Psychology of Music , 44(6), 1431–1438. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735616636209  

Christopher, E. A., & Shelton, J. T. (2017). Individual Differences in Working Memory Predict the Effect of Music on Student Performance. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition , 6(2), 167–173. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.01.012  

Colle, H. A., & Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 15(1), 17–31. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(76)90003-7  

David, P., Kim, J.-H., Brickman, J. S., Ran, W., & Curtis, C. M. (2015). Mobile phone distraction while studying. New Media & Society , 17(10), 1661–1679. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444814531692  

de Groot, A. M. B. (2006). Effects of Stimulus Characteristics and Background Music on Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning and Forgetting. Language Learning , 56(3), 463–506. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00374.x  

de la Mora Velasco, E., & Hirumi, A. (2020). The effects of background music on learning: A systematic review of literature to guide future research and practice. Educational Technology Research and Development , 68(6), 2817–2837. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09783-4  

Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology , 11(5), 445–455. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199710)11:5<445::AID-ACP472>3.0.CO;2-R  

Goltz, F., & Sadakata, M. (2021). Do you listen to music while studying? A portrait of how people use music to optimize their cognitive performance. Acta Psychologica , 220, 103417. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103417  

Groot, A. M. B. de, & Smedinga, H. E. (2014). LET THE MUSIC PLAY!: A Short-Term but No Long-Term Detrimental Effect of Vocal Background Music with Familiar Language Lyrics on Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 36(4), 681–707. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000059  

Hallam, S., & Godwin, C. (2015). Actual and perceived effects of background music on creative writing in the primary classroom. Psychology of Education Review , 39(2), 15–21. DOI: https://doi.org/10.53841/bpsper.2015.39.2.15  

Henninger, F., Shevchenko, Y., Mertens, U. K., Kieslich, P. J., & Hilbig, B. E. (2022). lab.js: A free, open, online study builder. Behavior Research Methods , 54(2), 556–573. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01283-5  

Henninger, F., Shevchenko, Y., Mertens, U., Kieslich, P. J., & Hilbig, B. E. (2020). lab.js: A free, open, online experiment builder. Zenodo . DOI: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fqr49  

Jäncke, L., Brügger, E., Brummer, M., Scherrer, S., & Alahmadi, N. (2014). Verbal learning in the context of background music: No influence of vocals and instrumentals on verbal learning. Behavioral and Brain Functions , 10(1), 10. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-10-10  

Jäncke, L., & Sandmann, P. (2010). Music listening while you learn: No influence of background music on verbal learning. Behavioral and Brain Functions , 6(1), 3. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-3  

Jones, D. M., & Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 19(2), 369–381. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.2.369  

Kämpfe, J., Sedlmeier, P., & Renkewitz, F. (2011). The impact of background music on adult listeners: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Music , 39(4), 424–448. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610376261  

Kiss, L., & Linnell, K. J. (2022). Making sense of background music listening habits: An arousal and task-complexity account. Psychology of Music , 03057356221089017. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/03057356221089017  

Küssner, M. B., Groot, A. M. B. de, Hofman, W. F., & Hillen, M. A. (2016). EEG Beta Power but Not Background Music Predicts the Recall Scores in a Foreign-Vocabulary Learning Task. PLOS ONE , 11(8), e0161387. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0161387  

LeCompte, D. C., Neely, C. B., & Wilson, J. R. (1997). Irrelevant speech and irrelevant tones: The relative importance of speech to the irrelevant speech effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 23, 472–483. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.2.472  

Linek, S. B., Marte, B., & Albert, D. (2011). Background Music in Educational Games: Motivational Appeal and Cognitive Impact. International Journal of Game-Based Learning (IJGBL) , 1(3), 53–64. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4018/ijgbl.2011070104  

Lo-Fi Study Music: Definition & Best Playlist | StudySmarter. (n.d.). StudySmarter UK. Retrieved October 17, 2022, from https://www.studysmarter.co.uk/magazine/lofi-study-music/  

Manthei, M., & Kelly, S. N. (1999). Effects of Popular and Classical Background Music on Math Test Scores of Undergraduate Students. Research Perspectives in Music Education , 6(1), 38–42.  

Miller, L. K., & Schyb, M. (1989). Facilitation and Interference by Background Music. Journal of Music Therapy , 26(1), 42–54. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/26.1.42  

Morey, C. C. (2018). The case against specialized visual-spatial short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin , 144(8), 849–883. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000155  

Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Reason , 4(2), 61–64. DOI: https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061  

Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs (0.9.12-2). http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor  

Mowsesian, R., & Heyer, M. R. (1973). The Effect of Music as a Distraction on Test-Taking Performance. Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance , 6(2), 104–110. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00256307.1973.12022580  

Nguyen, T., & Grahn, J. A. (2017). Mind your music: The effects of music-induced mood and arousal across different memory tasks. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain , 27, 81–94. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000178  

Nittono, H. (1997). Background Instrumental Music and Serial Recall. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 84(3_suppl), 1307–1313. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1307  

Overkott, C., & Souza, A. S. (2022). Verbal descriptions improve visual working memory but have limited impact on visual long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 151(2), 321–347. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001084  

Proverbio, A. M., Benedetto, F. D., Ferrari, M. V., & Ferrarini, G. (2018). When listening to rain sounds boosts arithmetic ability. PLOS ONE , 13(2), e0192296. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0192296  

R core team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/  

Reaves, S., Graham, B., Grahn, J., Rabannifard, P., & Duarte, A. (2016). Turn Off the Music! Music Impairs Visual Associative Memory Performance in Older Adults. The Gerontologist , 56(3), 569–577. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnu113  

Rentfrow, P. J. (2012). The Role of Music in Everyday Life: Current Directions in the Social Psychology of Music. Social and Personality Psychology Compass , 6(5), 402–416. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00434.x  

RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, PBC. URL http://www.rstudio.com/  

Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 21(2), 150–164. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90521-7  

Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. (2013). The effects of irrelevant speech on immediate free recall. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society , 28(6), 540–542. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334073  

Smith, C. A., & Morris, L. W. (1977). Differential Effects of Stimulative and Sedative Music on Anxiety, Concentration, and Performance. Psychological Reports , 41(3_suppl), 1047–1053. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1977.41.3f.1047  

Soares, A. P., Costa, A. S., Machado, J., Comesaña, M., & Oliveira, H. M. (2017). The Minho Word Pool: Norms for imageability, concreteness, and subjective frequency for 3,800 Portuguese words. Behavior Research Methods , 49(3), 1065–1081. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0767-4  

Souza, A. S., & Barbosa, L. C. L. (2023, April 17). Should We Turn off the Music? Lyrical Music Interferes with Cognitive Tasks . DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/XCV6E  

Souza, A. S., Rerko, L., Lin, H.-Y., & Oberauer, K. (2014). Focused attention improves working memory: Implications for flexible-resource and discrete-capacity models. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , 76(7), 2080–2102. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0687-2  

Su, Q., & Wang, F. (2010). Study the Effect of Background Music on Cognitive Memory. Applied Mechanics and Materials , 37–38, 1368–1371. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/AMM.37-38.1368  

Sutterer, D. W., & Awh, E. (2016). Retrieval practice enhances the accessibility but not the quality of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 23(3), 831–841. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0937-x  

The Benefits of Studying to Lo-Fi Music. (2021, October 25). Vaughn College . https://www.vaughn.edu/blog/studying-to-lo-fidelity-lo-fi-music-gets-high-marks-with-students/ .  

Tucker, A., & Bushman, B. J. (1991). Effects of Rock and Roll Music on Mathematical, Verbal, and Reading Comprehension Performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 72(3), 942–942. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1991.72.3.942  

Uittenhove, K., Jeanneret, S., & Vergauwe, E. (2023). From Lab-Testing to Web-Testing in Cognitive Research: Who You Test is More Important than how You Test. Journal of Cognition , 6(1), Article 1. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.259  

Vasilev, M. R., Hitching, L., & Tyrrell, S. (2022). What makes background music distracting? Investigating the role of song lyrics using self-paced reading . PsyArXiv. DOI: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/nmdt3  

Vasilev, M. R., Kirkby, J. A., & Angele, B. (2018). Auditory Distraction During Reading: A Bayesian Meta-Analysis of a Continuing Controversy. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 13(5), 567–597. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617747398  

Vilhena, D. de A., Sucena, A., Castro, S. L., & Pinheiro, Â. M. V. (2016). Reading Test—Sentence Comprehension: An Adapted Version of Lobrot’s Lecture 3 Test for Brazilian Portuguese. Dyslexia , 22(1), 47–63. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1521  

Winston, E., & Saywood, L. (2019). Beats to Relax/Study To: Contradiction and Paradox in Lofi Hip Hop. IASPM Journal , 9(2), Article 2. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5429/2079-3871(2019)v9i2.4en  

Wolf, R. H., & Weiner, F. F. (1972). Effects of Four Noise Conditions on Arithmetic Performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 35(3), 928–930. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1972.35.3.928  

Wolfe, D. E. (1983). Effects of Music Loudness on Task Performance and Self-Report of College-Aged Students. Journal of Research in Music Education , 31(3), 191–201. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/3345172  

Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature , 453(7192), 233–235. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06860  

IMAGES

  1. Music Whilst Doing Homework

    should students listen to music while doing homework

  2. Should students listen to music while doing homework?

    should students listen to music while doing homework

  3. Is Listening to Music while doing Homework OK: 21 best Songs

    should students listen to music while doing homework

  4. Listening to Music While Doing Homework and Studying: Is It A Good Idea

    should students listen to music while doing homework

  5. Handsome Student Listening To Music while Doing Homework Stock Image

    should students listen to music while doing homework

  6. Should Your Foster Kids Listen to Music While Studying?

    should students listen to music while doing homework

VIDEO

  1. When I listen to music while doing homework💀. #shorts

  2. Me casually listening to music while doing homework #funny #memes

  3. songs to listen to while doing math homework, a playlist

  4. 🎵Should you LISTEN MUSIC while STUDYING?!🤯 #jee #motivation

  5. K-pop songs i play while doing homework (I don't recommend it to people who get distracted easily)

  6. 𝐏𝐥𝐚𝐲𝐥𝐢𝐬𝐭 Lo-fi music while doing homework✏️ / 1hour Lofi mix / Chill music / Beats for study

COMMENTS

  1. Music and Studying: Do They Go Together? - Healthline

    Listening to music while you study or work doesnt always make you less productive or efficient. If you prefer to study with music, there’s no need to give it up.

  2. Listening to Music While Studying - The Pros & Cons [Upd. 2024]

    Explore the benefits and drawbacks of listening to music while studying. Learn how music can impact productivity, focus, and learning.

  3. Curious Kids: is it OK to listen to music while studying?

    Research suggest it’s probably fine to listen to music while you’re studying - with some caveats.

  4. Does Listening to Music Really Help You Study?

    Although listening to music can make studying more enjoyable, psychologists from the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences have found that this popular study habit is more distracting than beneficial.

  5. The Effects of Music on a Student's Schoolwork - Seattle PI

    Today's teens find it hard to resist listening to music while doing homework. Those who choose to listen while they study could see grades dip as a result. Teens need to...

  6. Does Listening to Music While Doing Homework Affect Your ...

    Students who listen to music while studying will be better at recalling the information they've learned if they also listen to music during tests -- an opportunity most students...

  7. Does Music Boost Your Cognitive Performance? | Scientific ...

    The fact that music can make a difficult task more tolerable may be why students often choose to listen to it while doing their homework or studying for exams. But is listening to...

  8. Do you listen to music while studying? A portrait of how ...

    Many studies report detrimental effects of BGM on reading, where participants perform worse on a reading comprehension task when they simultaneously listen to music, compared to their performance in a silent condition. However, a number of factors have been found to modulate this effect.

  9. Should We Turn off the Music? Music with Lyrics Interferes ...

    People often listen to music while doing cognitive tasks. Yet, whether music harms or helps performance is still debated. Here, we assessed the objective and subjective effects of music with and without lyrics on four cognitive tasks.

  10. Studying With Music: Arguments for & Against - E-Student

    According to relevant science, listening to music while studying does have its advantages as well as significant disadvantages – one thing is for sure, there is no concrete “yes or no” answer to whether music affects studying positively.