The Experience of Social Mobility: Social Isolation, Utilitarian Individualism, and Social Disorientation

  • Published: 25 May 2016
  • Volume 133 , pages 15–30, ( 2017 )

Cite this article

thesis on social mobility

  • Stijn Daenekindt 1 , 2  

3268 Accesses

37 Citations

1 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The dissociative thesis states that social mobility is a disruptive and detrimental experience for the individual. Despite the absence of convincing evidence either for or against it, this thesis is generally accepted in sociology. I investigate this thesis by considering three dimensions of dissociation—i.e., social isolation, utilitarian individualism, and social disorientation. I use data from a large-scale survey in Flanders (Belgium) and apply Diagonal Reference Models to study consequences of intergenerational social mobility. I find support for asymmetric acculturation for each dimension, i.e., upwardly mobile individuals adapt more to the new social status position, compared to downwardly mobile individuals. Moreover, both for social disorientation and utilitarian individualism, I find detrimental effects of the experience of downward social mobility. As I find no detrimental consequences of both upward and downward mobility, the results do not provide evidence for the dissociative thesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

thesis on social mobility

Social Mobility and Life Satisfaction across European Countries: A Compositional Perspective on Dissociative Consequences of Social Mobility

thesis on social mobility

What are the Trends and Explanations of Perceived Social Mobility in Poland?

thesis on social mobility

Understanding the Nature and Consequences of Social Mobility Beliefs

Sobel originally termed these models Diagonal Mobility Models, but De Graaf and Ganzeboom ( 1990 ) argued that Diagonal Reference Models was more appropriate. Currently, Diagonal Reference Models is used to refer to the models developed by Sobel.

Sorokin mentions additional specific dimensions of dissociation, e.g., “superficiality,” “cynicism,” “the hunt for sensual pleasure”. However, an overarching framework on the exact nature of dissociation is lacking in his writing.

The concept of social disorientation aligns with the concepts of “purposelessness” (Dean 1961 ) and “meaninglessness” (Seeman 1975 ).

Models were also estimated using two alternative operationalizations of social position of origin, i.e., “educational level of the father” and “highest educational level of the parents”. These analyses result in the same conclusions as the analyses presented here.

Diagonal Reference Models are also applied to study effects of other forms of status inconsistency, for example, heterogamy (e.g., Eeckhaut et al. 2013 ; Sorenson and Brownfield 1991 ; van der Slik et al. 2002 ).

Ashford, S. (1990). Upward mobility, status inconsistency, and psychological health. The Journal of Social Psychology, 130 (1), 71–76.

Article   Google Scholar  

Bean, F. D., Bonjean, C. M., & Burton, M. G. (1973). Intergenerational occupational mobility and alienation. Social Forces, 52 (1), 62–73.

Bean, F. D., & Swicegood, G. (1979). Intergenerational occupational mobility and fertility: A reassessment. American Sociological Review, 44 (4), 608–619.

Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life . New York: Harper and Row Publishers.

Google Scholar  

Berent, J. (1952). Fertility and social mobility. Population Studies, 5 (3), 244–260.

Blalock, H. (1967). The identification problem and theory building: The case of status inconsistency. American Sociological Review, 31 (1), 52–61.

Blau, P. (1956). Social mobility and interpersonal relations. American Sociological Review, 21 (3), 290–295.

Bourdieu, P. (2000). Pascalian meditations . Cambridge: Polity Press.

Bourdieu, P. (2007). Sketch for a self-analysis . Cambridge: Polity Press.

Clifford, P., & Heath, A. F. (1993). The political consequence of social mobility. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series A. Statistics in Society, 156 (1), 51–61.

Cooley, C. H. ([1909] 1983). Social organization: A study of the larger mind . New York: Schocken Books.

Coser, R. (1975). The complexity of roles as a seedbed of individual autonomy. In R. Coser (Ed.), The idea of social structure: Paper in honor of Robert K. Merton (pp. 237–263). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Coser, R. (1991). In defense of modernity: Role complexity and individual autonomy . Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Cox, D. R. (1990). Role of models in statistical analysis. Statistical Science, 5 (2), 169–174.

Daenekindt, S., & Roose, H. (2013a). Cultural chameleons: Social mobility and cultural practices in the private and the public sphere. Acta Sociologica, 56 (4), 309–324.

Daenekindt, S., & Roose, H. (2013b). A mise-en-scène of the shattered habitus: The effect of social mobility on aesthetic dispositions towards films. European Sociological Review, 29 (1), 48–59.

Daenekindt, S., & Roose, H. (2014). Social mobility and cultural dissonance. Poetics, 42 , 82–97.

De Graaf, N. D., & Ganzeboom, H. B. G. (1990). Cultuurdeelname en opleiding: Een analyse van statusgroep-effecten met Diagonale Referentiemodellen. Mens en Maatschappij, 65 (3), 219–245.

De Graaf, N. D., Nieuwbeerta, P., & Heath, A. (1995). Class mobility and political preferences: Individual and contextual effects. American Journal of Sociology, 100 (4), 997–1027.

Dean, D. G. (1961). Alienation: Its meaning and measurement. American Sociological Review, 26 (5), 753–758.

Duncan, O. D. (1966). Methodological issues in the analysis of social mobility. In N. Smelser & S. Lipset (Eds.), Social structure and mobility in economic development . Chicago: Aldine.

Durkheim, E. ([1897] 1930). Le suicide: Étude de sociologie . Paris: Alcan.

Eeckhaut, M. C., Van de Putte, B., Gerris, J. R., & Vermulst, A. A. (2013). Analysing the effect of educational differences between partners: A methodological/theoretical comparison. European Sociological Review, 29 (1), 60–73.

Ellis, R. A., & Lane, W. C. (1967). Social mobility and social isolation: A test of Sorokin’s dissociative hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 32 (2), 237–253.

Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24 (1), 41–74.

Franceschelli, M., Evans, K., & Schoon, I. (2016). ‘A fish out of water?’ The therapeutic narratives of class change. Current Sociology, 64 (3), 353–372.

Friedman, S. (2012). Cultural omnivores or culturally homeless? Exploring the shifting cultural identities of the upwardly mobile. Poetics, 40 (5), 467–489.

Friedman, S. (2014). The price of the ticket: Rethinking the experience of social mobility. Sociology, 48 (2), 352–368.

Friedman, S. (2015). Habitus clivé and the emotional imprint of social mobility. The Sociological Review, 64 (1), 129–147.

Geyer, F. (1974). Alienation and general systems theory. Sociologia Neerlandica, 10 , 18–41.

Goldthorpe, J. H. (1980). Social mobility and class structure in modern Britain (2nd ed.). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Hendrickx, J., De Graaf, N. D., Lammers, J., & Ultee, W. (1993). Models for status inconsistency and mobility: A comparison of the approaches by Hope and Sobel with the mainstream square additive model. Quality & Quantity, 27 (4), 335–352.

Hirtenlehner, H., Farrall, S., & Bacher, J. (2013). Culture, institutions, and morally dubious behaviors: Testing some core propositions of the institutional-anomie theory. Deviant Behavior, 34 (4), 291–320.

Hope, K. (1971). Social mobility and fertility. American Sociological Review, 36 (6), 1019–1032.

Hope, K. (1975). Models of status inconsistency and social mobility effects. American Sociological Review, 40 (3), 322–343.

Houle, J. N. (2011). The psychological impact of intragenerational social class mobility. Social Science Research, 40 (3), 757–772.

Houle, J. N., & Martin, M. A. (2011). Does intergenerational mobility shape psychological distress? Sorokin revisited. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 29 (2), 193–203.

Jackman, M. R. (1972). Social mobility and attitude toward the political system. Social Forces, 50 (4), 462–472.

Jackson, E. F. (1962). Status consistency and symptoms of stress. American Sociological Review, 27 (4), 469–480.

Jessor, R., Graves, T., Hanson, R. C., & Jessor, S. L. (1968). Society, personality and deviant behavior . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Kasarda, J. D., & Billy, J. O. (1985). Social mobility and fertility. Annual Review of Sociology, 11 , 305–328.

Kessin, K. (1971). Social and psychological consequences of intergenerational occupational mobility. American Journal of Sociology, 77 (1), 1–18.

Kingston, P. W., Hubbard, R., Lapp, B., Schroeder, P., & Wilson, J. (2003). Why education matters. Sociology of Education, 76 , 53–70.

Knoke, D. (1973). Intergenerational occupational mobility and the political party preferences of American men. American Journal of Sociology, 78 (6), 1448–1468.

Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment. American Journal of Sociology, 84 (1), 24–52.

Lenski, G. E. (1954). Status crystallization: A non-vertical dimension of social status. American Sociological Review, 19 (4), 405–413.

Lenski, G. E. (1956). Social participation and status crystallization. American Sociological Review, 21 (4), 458–464.

Lievens, J., Waege, H., & De Meulemeester, H. (2006). Cultuurkijker. Cultuurparticipatie gewikt en gewogen. Basisgegevens van de survey “Cultuurparticipatie in Vlaanderen 2003–2004” . Antwerp: De Boeck.

Lipset, S. M., & Bendix, R. (1959). Social mobility in industrial society . Berkeley: University of California Press.

Litwak, E. (1960). Occupational mobility and extended family cohesion. American Sociological Review, 25 (3), 9–21.

Lopreato, J. (1967). Upward social mobility and political orientation. American Sociological Review, 32 (4), 586–592.

Lopreato, J., & Chafetz, J. S. (1970). The political orientation of skidders: A middle-range theory. American Sociological Review, 35 (3), 440–451.

Luckmann, T., & Berger, P. (1964). Social mobility and personal identity. European Journal of Sociology, 5 (02), 331–344.

Marshall, G., & Firth, D. (1999). Social mobility and personal satisfaction: Evidence from ten countries. The British Journal of Sociology, 50 (1), 28–48.

McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Brashears, M. E. (2006). Social isolation in America: Changes in core discussion networks over two decades. American Sociological Review, 71 (3), 353–375.

Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure . New York: Free Press.

Meyer, J. W. (1977). The effects of education as an institution. American Journal of Sociology, 83 , 55–77.

Middleton, R. (1963). Alienation, race, and education. American Sociological Review, 28 (6), 973–977.

Mirande, A. M. (1973). Social mobility and participation: The dissociative and socialization hypotheses. The Sociological Quarterly, 14 (1), 19–31.

Missinne, S., Daenekindt, S., & Bracke, P. (2015). The social gradient in preventive healthcare use: What can we learn from socially mobile individuals? Sociology of Health & Illness, 37 (6), 823–838.

Monden, C. W. S., & de Graaf, N. D. (2013). The importance of father’s and own education for self-assessed health across Europe: An East-West divide? Sociology of Health & Illness, 35 (7), 977–992.

Newman, K. S. (1989). Falling from grace: The experience of downward mobility in the American middle class . New York: Vintage Books.

Nieuwbeerta, P., & De Graaf, N. D. (1993). Intergenerational class mobility and political preferences in the Netherlands between 1970 and 1986. Netherlands Journal of Social Sciences, 29 (3), 28–45.

Nieuwbeerta, P., De Graaf, N. D., & Ultee, W. (2000). The effects of class mobility on class voting in post-war Western industrialized countries. European Sociological Review, 16 (4), 327–348.

Putnam, R. D. (2007). E pluribus unum: Diversity and community in the twenty-first century the 2006 Johan Skytte Prize Lecture. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30 (2), 137–174.

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1 (3), 385–401.

Reay, D. (2013). Social mobility, a panacea for austere times: Tales of emperors, frogs, and tadpoles. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 34 (5–6), 660–677.

Reynolds, J. R., & Baird, C. L. (2010). Is there a downside to shooting for the stars? Unrealized educational expectations and symptoms of depression. American Sociological Review, 75 (1), 151–172.

Rubington, E., & Weinberg, M. (1995). The study of social problems: Seven perspectives . New York: Exford University Press.

Scott, W. (1958). Fertility and social mobility among teachers. Population Studies, 11 (3), 251–261.

Seeman, M. (1959). On the meaning of alienation. American Sociological Review, 24 (6), 783–791.

Seeman, M. (1975). Alienation studies. Annual Review of Sociology, 1 (1), 91–123.

Seeman, M. (1977). Some real and imaginary consequences of social mobility: A French-American comparison. American Journal of Sociology, 52 (4), 757–782.

Segal, D. R., & Knoke, D. (1968). Social mobility, status inconsistency and partisan realignment in the United States. Social Forces, 47 (2), 154–157.

Simpson, M. E. (1970). Social mobility, normlessness and powerlessness in two cultural contexts. American Sociological Review, 35 (6), 1002–1013.

Sobel, M. (1981). Diagonal Mobility Models—A substantively motivated class of designs for the analysis of mobility effects. American Sociological Review, 46 (6), 893–906.

Sobel, M. E. (1985). Social mobility and fertility revisited: Some new models for the analysis of the mobility effects hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 50 (5), 699–712.

Sorenson, A. M. (1989). Husbands’ and wives’ characteristics and fertility decisions: A Diagonal Mobility Model. Demography, 26 (1), 125–135.

Sorenson, A. (1994). Women, family and class. Annual Review of Sociology, 20 , 27–47.

Sorenson, A. M., & Brownfield, D. (1991). The measurement of parental influence: Assessing the relative influence of father and mother. Sociological Methods and Research, 19 (4), 511–535.

Sorokin, P. (1927). Social mobility . New York: Harper and Brothers.

Spruyt, B. (2015). Talent, effort or social background? An empirical assessment of popular explanations for educational outcomes. European Societies, 17 (1), 94–114.

Stevens, G. (1981). Social mobility and fertility: Two effects in one. American Sociological Review, 46 (5), 573–585.

Stuckert, R. P. (1963). Occupational mobility and family relationships. Social Forces, 41 (3), 301–307.

Thompson, K. H. (1971). Upward social mobility and political orientation: A re-evaluation of the evidence. American Sociological Review, 36 (2), 223–235.

Tolsma, J., De Graaf, N. D., & Quillian, L. (2009). Does intergenerational social mobility affect antagonistic attitudes towards ethnic minorities? British Journal of Sociology, 60 (2), 257–277.

Tumin, M. M. (1957). Some unapplauded consequences of social mobility in a mass society. Social Forces, 36 , 32.

Van der Slik, F. W., De Graaf, N. D., & Gerris, J. R. (2002). Conformity to parental rules: Asymmetric influences of father’s and mother’s levels of education. European Sociological Review, 18 (4), 489–502.

Van Der Waal, J., & De Koster, W. (2014). Occupational mobility. In: A.C. Michalos (Ed.), Encyclopedia of quality of life and well - being research . doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5 .

Vorwaller, D. J. (1970). Social mobility and membership in voluntary associations. American Journal of Sociology, 75 (4), 481–495.

Warner, W. L., & Abegglen, J. C. (1963). Big business leaders in America (Vol. 21). New York: Atheneum.

Weakliem, D. L. (1992). Does social mobility affect political behaviour? European Sociological Review, 8 (2), 153–165.

Wilensky, H. L. (1966). Measures and effects of social mobility. In N. Smelser & S. Lipset (Eds.), Social structure and mobility in economic development . Chicago: Aldine.

Wilensky, H. L., & Edwards, H. (1959). The skidder: Ideological adjustments of downward mobile workers. American Sociological Review, 24 (2), 215–231.

Willekens, M., Daenekindt, S., & Lievens, J. (2014). Whose education matters more? Mother’s and father’s education and the cultural participation of adolescents. Cultural Sociology, 8 (3), 291–309.

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am very grateful to Piet Bracke, Willem de Koster, Henk Roose and Jeroen van der Waal for kind and insightful comments on a previous version of this manuscript. I am also grateful to Guido Van Der Ratz for enlightening, and often strange, conversations.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Sociology, Ghent University, Korte Meer 5, 9000, Ghent, Belgium

Stijn Daenekindt

Department of Public Administration and Sociology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stijn Daenekindt .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Daenekindt, S. The Experience of Social Mobility: Social Isolation, Utilitarian Individualism, and Social Disorientation. Soc Indic Res 133 , 15–30 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-016-1369-3

Download citation

Accepted : 14 May 2016

Published : 25 May 2016

Issue Date : August 2017

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-016-1369-3

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Acculturation
  • Diagonal Reference Models
  • Dissociation
  • Dissociative thesis
  • Educational mobility
  • Social mobility
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Essays on Social Mobility

thesis on social mobility

--> Final eThesis - complete (pdf) -->

Filename: Bertha Rohenkohl _Final PhD Thesis.pdf

Creative Commons Licence

Embargo Date:

[img]

You do not need to contact us to get a copy of this thesis. Please use the 'Download' link(s) above to get a copy. You can contact us about this thesis . If you need to make a general enquiry, please see the Contact us page.

-

An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

PLOS ONE logo

Socioeconomic position, social mobility, and health selection effects on allostatic load in the United States

Alexi gugushvili, grzegorz bulczak, olga zelinska, jonathan koltai.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

* E-mail: [email protected]

Received 2021 Jan 31; Accepted 2021 Jun 25; Collection date 2021.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

The contemporaneous association between higher socioeconomic position and better health is well established. Life course research has also demonstrated a lasting effect of childhood socioeconomic conditions on adult health and well-being. Yet, little is known about the separate health effects of intergenerational mobility—moving into a different socioeconomic position than one’s parents—among early adults in the United States. Most studies on the health implications of mobility rely on cross-sectional datasets, which makes it impossible to differentiate between health selection and social causation effects. In addition, understanding the effects of social mobility on health at a relatively young age has been hampered by the paucity of health measures that reliably predict disease onset. Analysing 4,713 respondents aged 25 to 32 from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health’s Waves I and IV, we use diagonal reference models to separately identify the effects of socioeconomic origin and destination, as well as social mobility on allostatic load among individuals in the United States. Using a combined measure of educational and occupational attainment, and accounting for individuals’ initial health, we demonstrate that in addition to health gradient among the socially immobile, individuals’ socioeconomic origin and destination are equally important for multi-system physiological dysregulation. Short-range upward mobility also has a positive and significant association with health. After mitigating health selection concerns in our observational data, this effect is observed only among those reporting poor health before experiencing social mobility. Our findings move towards the reconciliation of two theoretical perspectives, confirming the positive effect of upward mobility as predicted by the “rags to riches” perspective, while not contradicting potential costs associated with more extensive upward mobility experiences as predicted by the dissociative thesis.

1. Introduction

Socioeconomic position is a fundamental cause of health disparities [ 1 ]. Those occupying higher rungs on the socioeconomic ladder tend to experience lower rates of morbidity and mortality compared to those placed lower in the social hierarchy [ 2 , 3 ]. In addition to socioeconomic position attained in adulthood, socioeconomic origins exert significant and independent effects on later life health [ 4 , 5 ], reflecting the “long arm” of childhood circumstances [ 6 – 8 ]. The enduring effects of childhood circumstances are thought to represent the downstream consequences of cumulative advantage and disadvantage, whereby stresses and strains accrue over the life course to a greater extent among those in socially disadvantaged positions, setting in motion more rapid aging or weathering of biological systems under conditions of chronic adversity [ 9 , 10 ].

An unresolved question in social stratification and social epidemiological research is whether the movement between origin and destination socioeconomic positions, per se , influences health net of origin and destination effects. Because social mobility is linearly dependent on both social origin and destination, traditional regression frameworks are not able to separately estimate the effects of socioeconomic origin, destination, and social mobility simultaneously [ 11 – 13 ], calling into question much of the existing evidence [ 14 ]. While Sobel’s diagonal reference models overcome this methodological limitation [ 15 ], few studies have utilized this statistical approach when investigating the health effects of social mobility, particularly in the North American context. This represents an important gap in the literature given renewed scholarly interest in intergenerational transmission of (dis)advantages and declining social mobility in the United States [ 16 – 18 ].

1.1. Key theories on health consequences of social mobility

Two main theoretical perspectives predict, respectively, negative and positive health consequences of upward social mobility. Sorokin’s dissociative thesis views upward social mobility as a deviation from expected continuity associated with individuals’ social origins [ 19 ]. Adjusting to an unfamiliar socioeconomic environment, while also socially distancing from the familiar and more natural past environment, can be a major stress-inducing process compromising upwardly mobile individuals’ psychological and, consequently, physical health. In turn, an alternative perspective, so-called “rags to riches” thesis [ 20 ], suggests that upward social mobility could lead to better health outcomes by generating a sense of personal control, boosting psychological well-being from overcoming life course constraints, fostering healthy behaviours and lifestyles, and developing a health conducive sense of gratitude among the upwardly mobile individuals [ 21 – 25 ].

In recent years scholarly interest in health consequences of downward rather than upward social mobility has become particularly salient. This is in line with the post-liberal theory of social stratification which views social mobility primarily in terms of offspring attaining worse off living conditions than their parents did [ 26 ]. The so-called “falling from grace” thesis implies that downward social mobility leads to an undesirable loss of an ascribed socioeconomic position at birth and associated changes in practices, behaviours, and norms [ 27 , 28 ]. The perception of downward mobility as undeserved and unjust, together with the overall psychological maladjustment to a new environment, can precipitate chronic stress and thus compromise the health of downwardly mobile individuals [ 29 , 30 ]. Downward mobility may also increase the stress associated with financial hardship, a well-known correlate of physical and mental health [ 31 ]. Given these multiple perspectives, one of the goals of our study is to derive new evidence on the merits of the main health-related theories of social mobility.

1.2. Independent social mobility and health selection effects

An overview of the existing studies does not provide conclusive answers about social mobility effects on health, as many studies report null findings [ 14 , 32 ]. When significant associations of upward social mobility and health are identified, these links are not usually detrimental for health [ 20 ], while downward mobility in a number of studies was found to be damaging to health [ 33 ]. Inferring from these findings, however, is problematic because researchers use different indicators of mobility in socioeconomic position such as occupational class, status, education, and income. Moreover, social mobility research is characterised by at least two significant methodological constraints: first, relatively few studies use a statistical approach which is able to separately identify the relative importance of origin and destination socioeconomic position, while also isolating the effect of social mobility on health outcomes; and second, most studies rely on cross-sectional datasets, and are thus unable to differentiate between health selection and social causation effects when studying health implications of social mobility.

Referring to the first problem, many studies on the health consequences of social mobility continue to apply conventional regression approaches which usually differentiate social mobility trajectories by combining individuals’ origin and destination positions and subsequently comparing health outcomes between these mobility groups; alternatively, some scholars simply omit from models either origin or destination socioeconomic positions to produce an estimate for health effect of upward or downward social mobility [ 14 , 34 ]. These analytical strategies are useful if researchers primarily intend to identify the role of origin and/or destination socioeconomic position for individuals’ health, but they are unable to differentiate if health outcomes are independently affected by position and mobility effects. To mitigate this concern, researchers have proposed a special form of regression model that was developed to estimate the relative effects of two hierarchically ranked socioeconomic positions and the net effect of a movement between these two positions on the outcome variable of interest [ 11 ].

The second methodological concern implies addressing a likely bias stemming from health selection by which individuals’ initial poor and good health not only, respectively, limits upward and facilitates downward mobility, but also is causally related to later life health. If health before social mobility is not accounted for, any possible health effects of social mobility can be erroneously attributed to social causation rather than to the health selection effects [ 35 ]. The role of health selection has been shown to be stronger for a transition process from adolescence to adulthood than for later life course transitions [ 36 ]. Considering the importance of initial health for educational attainment and resultant success on labour market, an intriguing and underexplored question is what are the health implications for those individuals who, regardless of initial health constraints, still manage to experience upward social mobility? Both positive and negative consequences can be predicted as mobility for disadvantaged individuals might imply greater costs, yet they might also derive greater psychological benefits from overcoming barriers on their social mobility journeys [ 37 ].

1.3. Social mobility and health measures

Adding to complexity, social mobility’s effects might differ not only if initial health is accounted for, but also depending on the type of health measures used in studies. The most prevalent outcome variable employed by scholars, due to its wide availability in social surveys, is self-rated health [ 34 , 38 , 39 ]. Yet, self-rated health is not a perfect predictor of objective indicators of health and people might think of different aspects of wellbeing while assessing their health status [ 40 , 41 ]. Some studies also use depressive symptoms to identify possible effects of social mobility on health, but they are not able to capture any effects of social mobility on physical health [ 20 , 42 ]. Timing of death, on the other hand, can be considered as a reliable indicator of health outcome, but data on mortality is mainly useful for studying later life health based on panel/cohort surveys or register-based datasets [ 43 , 44 ]. In this study, we use allostatic load, an index of multi-system physiological dysregulation among individuals [ 32 ], to identify the health implications of social mobility. This measure takes into account various aspects of health and provides information on valid variation in health already at a relatively young age [ 45 , 46 ].

1.4. Heterogeneous position and mobility effects

Existing studies on health inequalities suggest that different sociodemographic and socioeconomic groups have vastly different levels of allostatic load. Individuals’ age, for instance, is strongly associated with allostatic load, which might imply that across individuals’ life course mobility effects on AL also vary. The recent evidence also suggests that mobility effects across European societies are more pronounced among young people than among the elderly [ 39 ]. One of the explanations for this could be that psychological and stress-related costs and benefits of social mobility experience have primarily short-term effects that dissipate later in the life course [ 36 ]. Existing research on the health consequences of social mobility also indicates that the origin socioeconomic position might matter more for women than for men, while, in turn, mobility effects are more salient for men than women [ 20 ]. Social origin, attained socioeconomic position, and mobility between origin and destination positions might have varying implications for various sociodemographic groups which are known to have vastly different health and wellbeing outcomes due to historical, institutional, and structural differences. Race is of central importance in these respects.

In the United States, racial and ethnic inequalities in health and illness are well documented [ 47 ], and a growing body of research demonstrates the myriad ways that structural racism contributes to these disparities [ 48 ]. In addition to contemporaneous inequities, Gaydosh and colleagues argue that the health effects of social mobility may hinge on the disproportionate stressors experienced by racialized groups [ 49 ]. The John Henryism hypothesis, for example, posits that African American individuals are exposed to myriad psychosocial adversities throughout the life course, and that the active coping strategies employed to cope with such exigencies result in biological wear and tear [ 50 , 51 ]. Recent studies also suggest African Americans from disadvantaged backgrounds form a “skin-deep resilience,” wherein a higher sense of control may lead to favourable psychological outcomes but greater physiological dysregulation [ 52 – 55 ].

1.5. The United States as a case study

The United States is an interesting case to study the health consequences of social mobility as it is characterized by widening socioeconomic inequalities [ 56 ] and declining levels of intergenerational social mobility [ 17 , 57 ]. The United States also has one of the most comprehensive panel datasets, described in detail below, which allows us to investigate socioeconomic origin and destination, social mobility, and selection effects on individuals’ health. The same dataset used in the present study has been previously employed to investigate different health aspects of social mobility, including adolescent stressful experiences [ 58 ], early adversity on later life health through psychosocial resources [ 59 ], and the effects of life course socioeconomic position on cardiovascular health [ 60 ]. In turn, our contribution to the relevant scholarship is that we study consequences of social mobility on health by (1) constructing a robust indicator of socioeconomic position for both individuals and their parents; (2) identifying the relative importance of origin and destination socioeconomic positions on health; (3) detecting any residual effects of social mobility; (4) testing if position and mobility effects differ by sociodemographic characteristics such as gender and race; and (5) examining the role of health selection in the observed associations.

2.1. Dataset

The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) is a representative longitudinal study of individuals in the United States who were adolescents in the beginning of the 1990s. The study started in 1994–95 with Wave I of the panel which included data on 20,745 adolescents aged 12 to 19. By the time of writing, in the latest publicly available Wave IV, conducted in 2007–2008, the number of interviewed participants declined to 15,701 (76% of the original sample) with their average age of 29. Attrition did not occur completely at random, but rather the main identified predictors of response in Wave IV were individuals’ gender, race, and parental education [ 61 ]. For this analysis, we use data from Waves I and IV. Due to cost-related considerations, in this study, we used the public-use version of Add Health with about 40% of respondents chosen randomly from the restricted-full sample. The main differences between the public and the restricted versions of Add Health arise due to confidentiality concerns. The full version of the dataset contains more sensitive information on respondents including their romantic relationships and DNA-related data. The later information is not of primary interest for our study, while the random mode of selection of participants for the public-use version of Add Health ensures that it is a representative survey data of the United States population of the relevant age. After list-wise deletion of observations with missing information, 4,713 individuals were available for our analysis.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. health outcome.

Numerous past studies used the Add Health to investigate the impact of socioeconomic position on physical [ 62 – 64 ] and mental [ 49 , 55 , 65 ] health outcomes. Our goal was to construct a measure that would reliably detect health status determined by individuals’ long-term socioeconomic conditions as well as their experienced stress levels from Add Health’s Wave I to Wave IV. In this regard, one of the most appropriate indicators is individuals’ allostatic load (further AL). AL identifies multidimensional physiological dysregulations that contribute to an onset of disease progression [ 66 ]. AL index may incorporate neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic, and cardiovascular system functioning and is a validated predictor of morbidity and mortality outcomes, especially at the earlier stages of life [ 67 ].

There are alternative approaches to construct AL and no consensus exists concerning which is the most appropriate method [ 68 ]. In this study, building on the previous research [ 32 , 69 , 70 ], we constructed AL index using biomarkers data from a blood test and medical examinations collected at Wave IV. Our AL index is based on seven biomarkers divided into five categories: (1) Lipid—Total to High-Density Lipoprotein Cholesterol; (2) Glucose—Glucose MG/DL; (3) Inflammation—C-reactive protein (CRP); (4) Body Mass Index (BMI); and (5) Cardiovascular–(5.1) systolic and (5.2) diastolic blood pressure and (5.3) resting heart rate. Our approach to constructing this measure is closely matched with the previous research in which AL is based on lipid and glucose metabolism, inflammation (C-reactive protein and fibrinogen), body fat deposition (body mass index and waist measurement) and cardiovascular measures [ 32 ]. We first separately z-transformed the described biomarkers and then estimated the mean score of these transformed biomarkers. Finally, we z-transformed the derived mean AL score. Another approach would be to flag the biomarkers if they are above a relevant medical threshold [ 71 ]. Our preferred measure, however, is more sensitive as it captures the full variation in individuals’ AL and therefore may help to identify individuals that will develop more serious health problems in the future. We consider this measure to be particularly appropriate for our study as it is able to capture even relatively small changes in young adults’ health. This is especially important in the context of social mobility where the sensitivity of health outcome measures has yielded in mixed results [ 32 , 72 ]. For more detailed descriptive information about each employed component of AL index, refer to supporting information, S1 Table .

2.2.2. Social origin, destination, and mobility variables

For individuals’ social origin variables, we utilised information on parental characteristics collected directly from parents at Wave I, while individuals’ social destination variables are constructed from their attained socioeconomic position at Wave IV. A difference between origin and destination variables was classified as upward or downward social mobility.

Out of available measures of socioeconomic position in Add Health, we focused on individuals’ and their parents’ educational and occupational attainment which aligns with prior social stratification research in the United States [ 73 ]. Education is a known predictor of individuals’ health [ 74 ] and since the average age of respondents at Wave IV is 29, most participants have completed their educational attainment [ 75 ]. Formal educational credentials, however, measured in the survey did not fully capture individuals’ socioeconomic position due to unobserved heterogeneity in, among other areas, specific skills, productivity, and the quality and type of education, especially considering the fragmented and stratified educational system of the United States [ 76 ]. Therefore, we also utilised information on individuals’ occupational attainment which is a validated proxy for their earnings, work autonomy, and job security [ 77 ]. Combining educational and occupational information allowed us to generate a robust measure of socioeconomic position with known links to health outcomes [ 78 ].

For parental education, we used the highest level of education obtained by parents [ 79 ]. For example, we rely on mothers’ education if father completed high school and mother completed college. To construct our measures, we collapsed 10 educational categories for parents and 13 educational categories for respondents into 5 categories. The difference in the number of categories arose due to additional postgraduate degrees for respondents. We coded education variables as follows: some high school and lower (= 1), completed high school (= 2), some college (= 3), completed college 4-year degree (= 4), and completed some postgraduate qualifications (= 5).

Next, based on the previous research [ 80 , 81 ], we created occupational attainment variables for parents and respondents. In the case of parents, occupational data consisted of 10 occupational groups. We used the highest level of occupation obtained by the parents to construct five occupational categories. This was done by taking the average Nam-Power-Boyd scale score [ 82 ] for each of the 10 occupational groups and collapsing them into 5 categories from having no occupation (= 1) to Nam-Power-Boyd scale score from 70 to 100 (= 5). Individuals’ occupational status was based on the Standard Occupational Classification codes converted into status scores based on the Nam-Power-Boyd scale. We created quintiles from the converted occupational status scores (the lowest status jobs = 1, the highest status jobs = 5).

Finally, to derive the index of socioeconomic position for parents and individuals, we combined educational and occupational attainment variables. This resulted in scores ranging from 2 to 10 points for the highest achieving individuals. To ensure that each mobility group had adequate representation, we collapsed the combined socioeconomic position scores into quintiles, where quintile 5 represents the top 20% (highest attainment based on educational and occupational status). From these combined measures we calculated intergenerational social mobility variables. We subtracted parental from individuals scores. This resulted in a mobility measure ranging from -4 to 4, where 0 represents the immobile group. For example, if the respondent achieved a score equal 5, highest attainment (top quintile) and parental attainment was equal to 4, the difference between the two scores produces one-step upward mobility.

To ensure sufficient variation we collapse two, three and four steps into a long-range mobility indicator, separately for upward and downward mobility, while one-step mobility represents short-range mobility. Fig 1 shows the distribution of social mobility patterns with the immobile group being the largest single category of individuals, yet approximately 39% of respondents experienced downward mobility compared with 32% for upward mobility. In the empirical analysis, we differentiate between short-range (one-step) mobility and long-range mobility (two-four-steps).

Fig 1. Social mobility trajectories.

Fig 1

Note : Number of observations—4713.

2.2.3. Confounders and initial health status

In all multivariable models, we accounted for respondents’ gender and age. Additionally, based on the previous research on predictors of health in the United States [ 83 ], in the main analyses we controlled for respondents’ race/ethnicity (White [non-Hispanic], Black [non-Hispanic], Hispanic, and other), the type of residential area (rural = 1), and marital status (1 = married) [ 84 ].

To address possible health selection effects due to initial health status which could affect both social mobility experience and Wave IV health, we first utilised information on respondents’ self-rated health at Wave I. More specifically, the respondents were asked the following question: “In general, how is your health?” which they could rate on from 1 (= poor) to 5 (= excellent) health. Second, we accounted for individuals’ initial BMI scores. Third, we constructed the chronic disease indicator based on a set of four questions: “Do you have difficulty using your hands, arms, legs, or feet because of a permanent physical condition?”; “Do you have a permanent physical condition involving a heart problem?”; “Do you have a permanent physical condition involving asthma?”; “Do you have a permanent physical condition involving other breathing difficulties?”. If the respondent answered yes to any of these questions the indicator equals 1 and 0 otherwise. Correlations between the outcome measure and selected Wave I health measures are presented in Supporting information , S2 Table . Although other Wave I health outcome variables were available in the dataset (e.g. health assessed by parents and depressive symptoms based on the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D scale) [ 85 ]), additional analysis in Supporting information , S3 Table , shows that the selected health measures at Wave I are robust predictors of AL at Wave IV. Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the described health outcome measure, confounding variables, and initial health status.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Note : Number of observations– 4713.

2.3. Statistical analysis

We used diagonal reference models (DRM) to identify associations between different socioeconomic positions and AL score, to assess the relative importance of origin and destination socioeconomic positions, and to estimate the consequences of social mobility as a deviation from what could be expected from predicted health status of immobile individuals in origin and destination socioeconomic groups. DRM design allows overcoming multicollinearity problem arising due to social mobility measures being calculated directly from origin and destination socioeconomic position variables. In other words, conventional statistical models cannot simultaneously include origin, destination, and mobility parameters.

DRM is argued to be one of the most suitable methods for estimating an effect of social mobility because it disentangles this effect from the origin and destination position effects. An extensive overview of this statistical method, its usefulness in modelling of social mobility effects, and a comparison with conventional regression approaches are described and demonstrated elsewhere [ 11 , 14 , 29 ]. The key aspect of DRM is that immobile individuals’ health is estimated by weighted mean values of AL for those who occupy diagonal cells in our two-dimensional five by five matrix (see Table 2 in Results’ section). After accounting for immobile individuals’ health, DRM estimates the relative strength of the effect of the origin socioeconomic position to that of own socioeconomic position and this so-called “weight” parameter takes values between 0 and 1. A higher value of this weight indicates a greater relative effect of destination characteristics on the outcome measure, AL score in our case. In the case when the weight parameter is equal to 0.5 it can be concluded that the origin and destination characteristics play an equally important role in determining AL.

Table 2. Mean AL levels by parental and individuals’ socioeconomic position.

Notes : 95% confidence intervals in parentheses, number of observations– 4713. Quintiles are derived from combined educational and occupational attainment for parents and individuals respectively.

To estimate possible effects of social mobility on AL in DRM, diagonal intercepts (values of outcome variable of immobile individuals) and weight parameter are jointly used to specify a cell-specific intercept for each off-diagonal cell (specific downward and upward mobility trajectories) in the two-dimensional mobility table. After predicting values for all off-diagonal cells, the DRM approach specifies the effect of social mobility over and above the value of AL conditioned by specific characteristics of origin and destination socioeconomic positions. Social mobility variables and associated point estimates, βs, in DRM approach could be interpreted in the same way as in a conventional regression model, a reference category being a group of individuals with the same socioeconomic position as their parents. To test if the effect of the social origin on AL and the health implications of social mobility varied by the individuals’ socioeconomic position, their social mobility experiences, and other sociodemographic characteristics, in subsequent models we also derived point estimates for interaction terms between the weight parameters, social mobility, and confounding variables.

Lastly, as described in the Introduction section, we attempted to consider health selection of individuals into social mobility trajectories, which may also explain the later life health outcomes. For this purpose, we accounted for individuals’ initial health at Add Health Wave I. We first controlled for initial health variables and then fitted DRMs separately for individuals with good and with poor initial health status. All DRM estimates are derived using “drm” module in Stata 16 statistical software [ 86 ]. For various empirical applications of DRM approach in different countries and contexts readers can refer to studies on, among other areas, redistribution preferences [ 87 ], likelihood of smoking [ 88 ], attitudes toward immigrants [ 89 ].

3.1. Social gradient and descriptive mobility effects

Table 2 presents the mean levels of AL by individuals’ origin and destination socioeconomic positions. The diagonal cells consist of AL score for intergenerationally immobile individuals, while the remaining cells above and below the diagonal show, respectively, the mean AL score among upwardly and downwardly mobile individuals. Expectedly, the mean AL score is higher for immobile individuals with low socioeconomic position (0.20, CI 0.11,0.29) in comparison to immobile individuals with a high socioeconomic position (-0.32, CI -0.42,-0.22). Off diagonal cells also suggest that the upwardly mobile individuals have lower AL levels. A similar but inverse pattern can be observed for those who experienced downward mobility. These associations could be partially explained by the fact that the upwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the bottom quintile, while downwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the highest quintile. The latter also suggests that upwardly and downwardly mobile individuals differ by their social origin and destination positions and to disentangle these position effects from social mobility effects, we employed the above-described statistical approach—DRM.

3.2. Origin and destination weights and multivariable mobility effects

Table 3 presents findings from multivariable DRM estimations. In all models, we accounted for individuals’ age and gender, while in full models we also controlled for individuals’ race/ethnicity, urban/rural divide, and marital status. The results confirm the general patterns observed in Table 2 . It is important to clarify that the coefficients for immobile individuals represent weighted mean values of AL for those who occupy diagonal cells in our two-dimensional five by five matrix. In all models, immobile individuals in the highest and lowest socioeconomic quintile, respectively, have significantly lower and higher AL scores than immobile individuals in the middle socioeconomic quintile.

Table 3. Point estimates from DRM on AL levels.

* p < 0.05,

** p < 0.01,

*** p < 0.001,

95% confidence intervals in parentheses.

The calculated weight parameters in Model 1 show that the relative importance of parental socioeconomic position (0.35, CI 0.19,0.51) is lower than the importance of individuals’ own socioeconomic position (0.65, CI 0.49,0.81), which means that almost twice as much variation in the outcome variable is explained by individuals’ destination than by their origin. However, after social mobility variables are introduced, especially when short- and long-range mobility experiences are disentangled in Models 4–5, individuals’ own socioeconomic position becomes roughly equal to the effect of parental socioeconomic position. Models also show that age and gender are associated with individuals’ AL level. In Model 3, being male and older by one year are both linked to worse health by, respectively, 0.31 (CI 0.25,0.36) and 0.05 (CI 0.03,0.07) standard deviations of AL score.

Model 2 shows that upward mobility is significantly and negatively associated with individuals’ AL score, while the variable for downward mobility has a positive sign but it is not statistically significant. These mobility effects are unaffected when further sociodemographic controls—individuals’ race/ethnicity, marital status, and urban/rural divide—are accounted for in Model 3. The results for these variables suggest that there are significant differences between Blacks and Whites, the former having more than a 0.18 (CI 0.11,0.25) higher AL score. Marital status does not play a significant role in explaining variation in our outcome measure but living in rural areas is associated with a higher AL score (0.08, CI 0.02,0.14). In Models 4–5, we disentangle social mobility variables into short- and long-range upward and downward mobility. Confidence intervals for three out of four social mobility variables overlap with zero, but a significant association is maintained for short-range upward mobility. Moving up by one socioeconomic quintile is linked with a 0.10 (CI -0.19,-0.01) decrease in individuals’ AL score.

In Supporting information , S4 Table , we estimated the effects of upward and downward mobility in reference to both immobility and mobility in the opposite direction by removing the latter coefficients from the fitted models. In S5 Table of supporting information we also present DRM estimates which account for neighbourhood characteristics where individuals lived at Wave I, such as poverty rates, unemployment levels, and race/ethnicity composition. These supplementary results are identical to those reported in the main analysis. Lastly, it is possible that the respondents are still too young to be certain that downward mobility will not change as time passes. This may be particularly true in terms of occupational attainment. To address this issue, in Supporting information , S6 Table , we estimate our models with education as the only SEP measure and educational mobility parameters. These results show no educational mobility effects, while in terms of health gradient and the importance of the relative weight, no major differences were observed.

3.3. Do origin and mobility effects vary by sociodemographic groups?

In Table 4 , we estimated DRMs with the interaction terms between the origin weight, social mobility, and a set of covariates—age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, and urban-rural divide. We did not find that any interaction terms between the social origin weight and the described parameters were statistically significant, which implied that the effect of origin socioeconomic position on individuals’ AL score did not vary by social mobility experiences and sociodemographic characteristics. In Table 4 , we also interacted upward and downward mobility experiences with gender, age, marital status, and race/ethnic variables to test if the effect of social mobility on AL score had varying implications for these sociodemographic groups. Past research on various health outcomes in the United States indicates that due to historical and structural factors, including discrimination, racial/ethnic differences in social mobility’s effect on AL may be present [ 84 , 90 ].

Table 4. Origin weights and point estimates for interaction terms from DRM on AL levels.

*** p < 0.001.

95% confidence intervals in parentheses, constitutive terms of interactions are not shown, number of observations– 4,713.

For none of these interaction terms, except marital status, we found statistically significant associations, which indicated that mobility effects on AL did not, as a rule, vary by the considered sociodemographic groups. It should be emphasised that these findings, mostly insignificant, should be interpreted with caution as for the selected interactions the sample size may be too small to provide sufficient variation and conclusive findings.

3.4. How does health selection matter?

We now address the possibility that individuals’ health before experiencing social mobility affected both mobility trajectories and their AL score. To account for individuals’ initial health status in our models, we used their self-rated health (a binary variable which equalled to 1 if respondents’ health was worse than very good), BMI levels (with middle quintile of BMI being the reference category), and having chronic health problems at Wave I (a dummy variable). First, we checked if initial health was associated with social mobility in Supporting information , S7 Table , and concluded that those with worse health had, respectively, lower and higher chances to experience upward and downward social mobility, even after the origin socioeconomic position was controlled for. This suggested that health selection has to be accounted for when studying the effects of social mobility on health. Results in Table 5 , Models 1 and 2, in turn, show that poor health and high BMI levels prior to social mobility experience were significantly and positively associated with AL score at Wave IV. In comparison to the results in Table 3 , we observed a decrease in the size of coefficients for the highest and lowest socioeconomic quintiles of immobile individuals. The earlier detected effect of short-range upward mobility became insignificant in these two models.

Table 5. Point estimates from DRM on AL levels with initial health.

Models 3 to 6 show the results where the total sample was split by individuals’ self-rated poor and good health at Wave I. This exercise created two groups of individuals, one with initial self-rated excellent/very good (69%) and another with worse than very good (31%) health. For individuals with very good and excellent initial health, AL score of immobile groups did not significantly change in comparison to the pooled sample, but for those with worse initial self-rated health social gradient in AL was less salient. More importantly, we found that short-range upward mobility had a significant negative association with AL score but only among individuals with initial poor health, even after the initial BMI level and chronic health problems were accounted for.

To further explore the role of initial health in the observed positive effect of upward mobility, in Supporting information , S8 Table , we interacted individuals’ upward and downward mobility trajectories with their initial health status. Results showed that the interaction term was negative and statistically significant only for those who experienced short-range upward mobility, which again confirmed the positive health implications of upward mobility for the less healthy individuals at Wave I.

4. Discussion

In this study, we explored how health outcomes of young adults in the United States are conditioned by individuals’ socioeconomic position and their movements between origin and destination, also accounting for initial health and health selection effects into specific mobility experiences. The allostatic load score used in this study is one of the most comprehensive measures of health, especially among relatively young individuals who tend to rate their health as excellent or very good in social surveys, rarely experience hospitalization, and have very low levels of mortality. While social gradient in health is not surprising and there is a number of studies investigating the role of various aspects of socioeconomic position for AL in the United States (some also using the same dataset as we did [ 10 , 60 , 68 , 70 ]), our paper makes an important scholarly contribution as it identifies the relative impact of social origin (parental characteristics) in comparison to the social destination (own attained socioeconomic position); detects possible independent effects of movements from parental to own socioeconomic position; tests if position and mobility effects differ by socioeconomic groups; and investigates the role of health selection in the observed associations.

Using Add Health data and DRM statistical approach, which was specifically designed to distinguish the effects of origin and destination socioeconomic positions from independent effects of mobility between these two, we showed that the combined measure of educational and occupational attainment is a robust predictor of biological markers of health. Those in higher and lower quantiles of socioeconomic position had respectively lower and higher levels of AL even when the standard sociodemographic factors such as age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, and rural-urban divide were accounted for. In addition to demonstrating socioeconomic gradient in AL, we revealed that when social mobility is accounted for, socioeconomic origins and destination are of almost equal importance for health. This finding is in line with the results from the United Kingdom where the effects of both parental and own socioeconomic position on individuals’ AL were roughly equal [ 32 ]. Yet, it contrasts with research on other health and wellbeing outcomes where origin characteristics are less important [ 80 , 91 ]. One explanation for this could be that our AL index based on the selected biomarkers including metabolic and cardiovascular system functioning, is more sensitive to lifetime exposures and experiences, while alternative measures such as, for instance, health-related behaviours and perceptions are more likely to be shaped by individuals’ contemporary conditions.

While exploring social mobility effects, we found that upward mobility is linked to lower levels of AL, however, in contrast to theoretical expectations [ 27 , 29 ], downward mobility was not associated with health. These are not completely novel findings, as similar positive effects of upward mobility were shown in the previous studies [ 20 , 92 ]. When we disaggregated mobility experiences in short- and long-range upward mobility, we found that the positive effect on health stemmed from the short-range upward mobility experience. The magnitude of the short-range upward mobility’ effect on AL is quite large and it is roughly comparable to the effect of living in urban areas or being two years younger, while for those with the initial poor health this effect is even larger. It is important to note that due to relatively young age composition of the analytical sample, for many individuals the downward social mobility observed in this study may be temporary, which could be one possible explanation why we find a null effect of downward mobility on AL.

Returning to the initial question posed in the introductory section of this study—which theoretical perspectives on the health implications of social mobility do these findings support? Our results are in line with predictions of “rags to riches” thesis which suggests that psychological benefits of overcoming constraints and moving up in social hierarchy may lead to lower levels of stress and positive life experiences which, in turn, can prevent an increase of AL among individuals. At the same time, our finding that the long-range upward mobility did not reduce AL score could indicate that more extensive mobility experiences may be associated with some negative health-related consequences, as predicted by the dissociation thesis. A more effortful upward mobility experience likely dilutes the positive health implications of moving up in the social hierarchy. This is supported by the observation that when we collapsed socioeconomic positions variables into tertiles rather than quintiles in S9 Table , supporting information, we did not see this positive effect of upward mobility on AL score because mobility between tertiles might simultaneously imply elements of short- and long-range social mobility.

We did not detect heterogeneous effects of social origins and mobility experiences across sociodemographic groups. Apparently, both parental characteristics and mobility from origin to destination have similar implications for health, irrespective of individuals’ personal characteristics. Our finding contrast with results from past research using the same dataset, suggesting that selected minority groups (Black and Hispanic individuals) experience higher metabolic syndrome after college completion [ 49 ]. Three main methodological aspects are likely to explain this difference. First, our research strategy is focused on disentangling mobility effect from origin and destination effects, based on SEP derived from educational and occupational attainment, and using DRM approach, while Gaydosh et al. rely only on educational mobility and use conventional Poisson regressions. Second, health measures also differ noticeably as Gaydosh et al. rely on metabolic syndrome based on blood pressure, glycosylated hemoglobin, body to waist ratio and cholesterol, while AL measure used in our study in addition to components such as blood pressure and cholesterol includes other biomarkers (i.e. CRP and BMI). Third, Gaydosh et al. use the restricted-full sample, while we use the public version of Add Health. These differences make any direct comparison across the two studies difficult.

Furthermore, we found that accounting for initial health, e.g. health selection, before experiencing social mobility mattered for health outcomes following upward mobility experiences. First, once initial health was controlled, the positive effect of the upward mobility became insignificant. However, when we disaggregated the analytical sample into two groups with good and poor initial health, upward mobility was significantly associated with lower AL score among individuals with poor initial health. One interpretation of this finding is that individuals who had some initial health problems, and presumably high levels of AL, particularly benefited psychologically from moving up in social hierarchy because, regardless of initial adverse conditions, they managed to overcome health-related constraints and consequently developed feelings of life control, achievement, and gratitude. This explanation is also supported by earlier evidence that education is more beneficial for health of those with the lowest propensity to attain higher education based on a wide range of childhood adversities [ 37 ].

Despite using a high-quality data set, a comprehensive indicator of socioeconomic position, a validated measure of individuals’ health, and DRM—a recommended method to identify the health consequences of social mobility, our study has its limitations. First, due to the age composition of Add Health dataset, we focused on the health implications of social mobility on individuals at a relatively early stage in terms of their life-time socioeconomic position. Although a recent study implies that social mobility might have only short-term consequences observed when mobile individuals are relatively young [ 39 ], it is likely that the Add Health cohort at Wave IV is still too young to fully differentiate between mobile and immobile individuals as they are expected to experience further upward or downward social mobility. This seems to be particularly important in terms of occupational attainment and it may be addressed in the future with Add Health’s Wave V data, when respondents are 10 years older. Wave V can also provide valuable health information allowing us re-examining the relationship between social mobility and AL of the relatively older cohort. Methodologically, it is important to note that although we mitigated the bias from confounding factors and health selection, our approach cannot identify a causal relationship between social mobility and AL.

The main conclusion of our study is that not only does socioeconomic position matter for young adults health in the United States, but also whether the attained status is the result of mobility between individuals’ origin and destination socioeconomic environments. This mobility effect, however, can be identified if an appropriate statistical approach is employed and individuals’ initial health is adequately accounted for. We also call for more nuanced consideration of the impact of short- and long-term mobility on health. The finding that the short-range upward social mobility is associated with better outcomes among those with poor childhood health suggests that social mobility might be a transformative experience for particularly disadvantaged individuals. Yet, our finding that there is no positive health effect of long-range upward mobility also indicates that more extensive mobility experiences might entail costs which cannot be compensated by positive implications of social mobility.

Supporting information

Note : Number of observations—4,713.

Notes : * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, 95% confidence intervals in parentheses.

Notes : * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, 95% confidence intervals in parentheses. Neighbourhood controls capture the most localized available contextual information (approx. 452 housing units, block groups) of the areas in which individuals lived at Wave IV. For modal neighbourhood race categories include white (ref.), black, other; poverty measure represents the proportion of persons with income below poverty level. Based on the distribution of proportion of persons below poverty level in 1989. Low = neighbourhoods where the proportion of the population with income below poverty level was less than 11.6%, the median proportion; medium = between 11.6% and 23.9%; and high (ref.) = above 23.9%; for unemployment rate: Low = neighbourhoods with an unemployment rate less than 6.5%, the median rate, medium = between 6.5% and 10.9%; high = 10.9% or higher.

Notes : * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. 95% confidence intervals in parentheses.

Data Availability

The data underlying the results presented in the study are available from Add Health https://addhealth.cpc.unc.edu/data/ .

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Polish National Science Centre grant received by AG (Program SONATA14) - https://ncn.gov.pl/ - [grant number UMO-2018/31/D/HS6/ 01877]. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

  • 1. Link BG, Phelan J. Social Conditions As Fundamental Causes of Disease. J Health Soc Behav. 1995;35: 80. doi: 10.2307/2626958 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 2. Phelan JC, Link BG, Tehranifar P. Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Health Inequalities: Theory, Evidence, and Policy Implications. J Health Soc Behav. 2010;51: S28–S40. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383498 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3. Marmot M. Social determinants of health inequalities. Lancet. 2005;365: 1099–1104. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)71146-6 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, Sethi D, Butchart A, Mikton C, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Heal. 2017;2: e356–e366. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Marmot M. The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Owl Book; 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Hayward MD, Gorman BK. The Long Arm of Childhood: The Influence of Early-Life Social Conditions on Men’s Mortality. Demography. 2004;41: 87–107. doi: 10.1353/dem.2004.0005 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 7. Haas S. Trajectories of functional health: The ‘long arm’ of childhood health and socioeconomic factors. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66: 849–861. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.004 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Tiikkaja S, Sandin S, Malki N, Modin B, Sparén P, Hultman CM. Social class, social mobility and risk of psychiatric disorder—A population-based longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2013;8: 1–9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077975 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Geronimus AT, Hicken M, Keene D, Bound J. “Weathering” and Age Patterns of Allostatic Load Scores Among Blacks and Whites in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2006;96: 826–833. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Gruenewald TL, Karlamangla AS, Hu P, Stein-Merkin S, Crandall C, Koretz B, et al. History of socioeconomic disadvantage and allostatic load in later life. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74: 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.09.037 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11. Sobel ME. Diagonal mobility models: A substantively motivated class of designs for the analysis of mobility effects. Am Sociol Rev. 1981;46: 893–906. doi: 10.2307/2095086 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Missinne S, Daenekindt S, Bracke P. The social gradient in preventive healthcare use: what can we learn from socially mobile individuals? Sociol Health Illn. 2015;37: 823–838. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12225 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Präg P, Gugushvili A. Intergenerational Social Mobility and Self-Rated Health in Europe. SocArxiv. 2020. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. van der Waal J, Daenekindt S, de Koster W. Statistical challenges in modelling the health consequences of social mobility: the need for diagonal reference models. Int J Public Health. 2017;62: 1029–1037. doi: 10.1007/s00038-017-1018-x [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Sobel ME. Social Mobility and Fertility Revisited. Some New Models for the Analysis of the Mobility Effects Hypothesis. Am Sociol Rev. 1985;50: 699–712. doi: 10.2307/2095383 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Chetty R, Hendren N, Kline P, Saez E, Turner N. Is the United States still a land of opportunity? Recent trends in intergenerational mobility. Am Econ Rev. 2014;104: 141–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Chetty R, Grusky D, Hell M, Hendren N, Manduca R, Narang J. The fading American dream: Trends in absolute income mobility since 1940. Science (80-). 2017;356: 398–406. doi: 10.1126/science.aal4617 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Hout M. Americans’ occupational status reflects the status of both of their parents. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: 9527–9532. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1802508115 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Sorokin PA. Social mobility. New York: Harper & Brothers; 1927. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. Gugushvili A, Zhao Y, Bukodi E. ‘Falling from grace’ and ‘rising from rags’: Intergenerational educational mobility and depressive symptoms. Soc Sci Med. 2019;222: 294–304. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.12.027 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Hill PL, Allemand M, Roberts BW. Examining the pathways between gratitude and self-rated physical health across adulthood. Pers Individ Dif. 2013;54: 92–96. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.011 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. Mirowsky J, Ross CE. Education, Social Status, and Health. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23. Poulton R, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Thomson WM, Taylor A, Sears MR, et al. Association between children’s experience of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: a life-course study. Lancet. 2002;360: 1640–1645. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11602-3 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LC. Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychol Inq. 2004;15: 1–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Watkins PC, Woodward K, Stone T, Kolts RL. Gratitude and happiness: Development of a measure of gratitude, and relationships with subjective well-being. Soc Behav Pers. 2003;31: 431–452. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2003.31.5.431 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Jackson M, Grusky DB. A post-liberal theory of stratification. Br J Sociol. 2018;69: 1096–1133. doi: 10.1111/1468-4446.12505 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 27. Newman KS. Falling from Grace: Downward Mobility in the Age of Affluence. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Gugushvili A. A multilevel analysis of perceived intergenerational mobility and welfare state preferences. Int J Soc Welf. 2019;28: 16–30. doi: 10.1111/ijsw.12316 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Houle JN, Martin MA. Does intergenerational mobility shape psychological distress? Sorokin revisited. Res Soc Stratif Mobil. 2011;29: 193–203. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2010.11.001 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Gugushvili A, McKee M, Murphy M, Azarova A, Irdam D, Doniec K, et al. Intergenerational Mobility in Relative Educational Attainment and Health-Related Behaviours. Soc Indic Res. 2019;141: 413–441. doi: 10.1007/s11205-017-1834-7 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Koltai J, Bierman A, Schieman S. Financial circumstances, mastery, and mental health: Taking unobserved time-stable influences into account. Soc Sci Med. 2018;202: 108–116. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.01.019 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Präg P, Richards L. Intergenerational social mobility and allostatic load in Great Britain. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2019;73: 100–105. doi: 10.1136/jech-2017-210171 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Nicklett EJ, Burgard S a. Downward social mobility and major depressive episodes among latino and Asian-American immigrants to the United States. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170: 793–801. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp192 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Campos-Matos I, Kawachi I. Social mobility and health in European countries: Does welfare regime type matter? Soc Sci Med. 2015;142: 241–248. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.08.035 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 35. Elstad JI, Krokstad S. Social causation, health-selective mobility, and the reproduction of socioeconomic health inequalities over time: panel study of adult men. Soc Sci Med. 2003;57: 1475–1489. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00514-2 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 36. Hoffmann R, Kröger H, Pakpahan E. Pathways between socioeconomic status and health: Does health selection or social causation dominate in Europe? Adv Life Course Res. 2018;36: 23–36. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2018.02.002 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 37. Schafer MH, Wilkinson LR, Ferraro KF. Childhood (Mis)fortune, Educational Attainment, and Adult Health: Contingent Benefits of a College Degree? Soc Forces. 2013;91: 1007–1034. doi: 10.1093/sf/sos192 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 38. Iveson MH, Deary IJ. Intergenerational social mobility and subjective wellbeing in later life. Soc Sci Med. 2017;188: 11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.06.038 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 39. Steiber N. Intergenerational educational mobility and health satisfaction across the life course: Does the long arm of childhood conditions only become visible later in life? Soc Sci Med. 2019;242: 112603. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112603 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 40. Krause NM, Jay GM. What Do Global Self-Rated Health Items Measure? Med Care. 1994;32: 930–942. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199409000-00004 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 41. Lorem G, Cook S, Leon DA, Emaus N, Schirmer H. Self-reported health as a predictor of mortality: A cohort study of its relation to other health measurements and observation time. Sci Rep. 2020;10: 4886. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61603-0 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 42. Timms D. Gender, social mobility and psychiatric diagnoses. Soc Sci Med. 1998;46: 1235–1247. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(97)10052-1 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 43. Billingsley S. Intragenerational social mobility and cause-specific premature mortality. PLoS One. 2019;14: 8–13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211977 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 44. Claussen B, Smits J, Naess O, Davey G. Intragenerational mobility and mortality in Oslo: Social selection versus social causation. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61: 2513–2520. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.045 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 45. Seeman TE, McEwen BS, Rowe JW, Singer BH. Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2001;98: 4770–4775. doi: 10.1073/pnas.081072698 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 46. Seeman M, Stein Merkin S, Karlamangla A, Koretz B, Seeman T. Social status and biological dysregulation: The “status syndrome” and allostatic load. Soc Sci Med. 2014;118: 143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.08.002 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 47. Williams DR, Mohammed SA, Leavell J, Collins C. Race, socioeconomic status, and health: Complexities, ongoing challenges, and research opportunities. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186: 69–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05339.x [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 48. Bailey ZD, Krieger N, Agénor M, Graves J, Linos N, Bassett MT. Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: evidence and interventions. Lancet. 2017;389: 1453–1463. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30569-X [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 49. Gaydosh L, Schorpp KM, Chen E, Miller GE, Harris KM. College completion predicts lower depression but higher metabolic syndrome among disadvantaged minorities in young adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: 109–114. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714616114 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 50. James SA, Hartnett SA, Kalsbeek WD. John Henryism and blood pressure differences among black men. J Behav Med. 1983;6: 259–278. doi: 10.1007/BF01315113 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 51. James SA, Keenan NL, Strogatz DS, Browning SR, Garrett JM. Socioeconomic Status, John Henryism, and Blood Pressure in Black Adults. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135: 59–67. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116202 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 52. Miller GE, Yu T, Chen E, Brody GH. Self-control forecasts better psychosocial outcomes but faster epigenetic aging in low-SES youth. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112: 10325–10330. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1505063112 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 53. Brody GH, Yu T, Chen E, Miller GE, Kogan SM, Beach SRH. Is Resilience Only Skin Deep?: Rural African Americans’ Socioeconomic Status–Related Risk and Competence in Preadolescence and Psychological Adjustment and Allostatic Load at Age 19. Psychol Sci. 2013;24: 1285–1293. doi: 10.1177/0956797612471954 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 54. Chen E, Miller GE, Brody GH, Lei M. Neighborhood Poverty, College Attendance, and Diverging Profiles of Substance Use and Allostatic Load in Rural African American Youth. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3: 675–685. doi: 10.1177/2167702614546639 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 55. Brody GH, Yu T, Miller GE, Chen E. Resilience in Adolescence, Health, and Psychosocial Outcomes. Pediatrics. 2016;138: e20161042. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1042 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 56. Zimmerman FJ, Anderson NW. Trends in Health Equity in the United States by Race/Ethnicity, Sex, and Income, 1993–2017. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2: e196386. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.6386 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 57. Song X, Massey CG, Rolf KA, Ferrie JP, Rothbaum JL, Xie Y. Long-term decline in intergenerational mobility in the United States since the 1850s. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2020;117: 251–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1905094116 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 58. Wickrama KAS, O’Neal CW, Lee TK. The Health Impact of Upward Mobility: Does Socioeconomic Attainment Make Youth More Vulnerable to Stressful Circumstances? J Youth Adolesc. 2016;45: 271–285. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0397-7 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 59. Wickrama K(K. AS., O’Neal CW, Lee TK, Wickrama T. Early socioeconomic adversity, youth positive development, and young adults’ cardio-metabolic disease risk. Heal Psychol. 2015;34: 905–914. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 60. Walsemann KM, Goosby BJ, Farr D. Life course SES and cardiovascular risk: Heterogeneity across race/ethnicity and gender. Soc Sci Med. 2016;152: 147–155. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.01.038 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 61. Brownstein N, Kalsbeek WD, Tabor J, Entzel P, Daza E, Harris KM. Non-Response in Wave IV of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 62. Chen E, Yu T, Siliezar R, Drage JN, Dezil J, Miller GE, et al. Evidence for skin-deep resilience using a co-twin control design: Effects on low-grade inflammation in a longitudinal study of youth. Brain Behav Immun. 2020. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.070 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 63. Yang YC, Gerken K, Schorpp K, Boen C, Harris KM. Early-Life Socioeconomic Status and Adult Physiological Functioning: A Life Course Examination of Biosocial Mechanisms. Biodemography Soc Biol. 2017. doi: 10.1080/19485565.2017.1279536 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 64. Belsky DW, Domingue BW, Wedow R, Arseneault L, Boardman JD, Caspi A, et al. Genetic analysis of social-class mobility in five longitudinal studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115: E7275–E7284. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801238115 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 65. Miller GE, Chen E, Yu T, Brody GH. Youth Who Achieve Upward Socioeconomic Mobility Display Lower Psychological Distress But Higher Metabolic Syndrome Rates as Adults: Prospective Evidence From Add Health and MIDUS. J Am Heart Assoc. 2020. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.015698 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 66. McEwen BS. Stress and the Individual: Mechanisms Leading to Disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153: 2093. doi: 10.1001/archinte.1993.00410180039004 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 67. Juster RP, McEwen BS, Lupien SJ. Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35: 2–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.002 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 68. Johnson SC, Cavallaro FL, Leon DA. A systematic review of allostatic load in relation to socioeconomic position: Poor fidelity and major inconsistencies in biomarkers employed. Soc Sci Med. 2017;192: 66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.025 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 69. McCrory C, Fiorito G, Ni Cheallaigh C, Polidoro S, Karisola P, Alenius H, et al. How does socio-economic position (SEP) get biologically embedded? A comparison of allostatic load and the epigenetic clock(s). Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;104: 64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.018 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 70. Vie TL, Hufthammer KO, Holmen TL, Meland E, Breidablik HJ. Is self-rated health a stable and predictive factor for allostatic load in early adulthood? Findings from the Nord Trøndelag Health Study (HUNT). Soc Sci Med. 2014;117: 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.07.019 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 71. Davillas A, Pudney S. Concordance of health states in couples: Analysis of self-reported, nurse administered and blood-based biomarker data in the UK Understanding Society panel. J Health Econ. 2017;56: 87–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2017.09.010 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 72. Präg P, Gugushvili A. Subjective social mobility and health in Germany. Eur Soc. 2020; 1–23. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 73. Bartley M. Health Inequality: An Introduction to Concepts, Theories and Methods. Second Edi. Cambridge: Polity Press; 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 74. Zajacova A, Lawrence EM. The Relationship Between Education and Health: Reducing Disparities Through a Contextual Approach. Annu Rev Public Health. 2018;39: 273–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044628 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 75. Bernardi F, Ballarino G, editors. Education, occupation and social origin: A comparative analysis of the transmission of socio-economic inequalities. Cheltenham, UK; Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing Limited; 2016. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 76. Torche F. Education and the intergenerational transmission of advantage in the US. In: Bernardi F, Ballarino G, editors. Education, Occupation and Social Origin. Edward Elgar Publishing; 2016. pp. 237–254. doi: 10.4337/9781785360459.00020 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 77. Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH. Intergenerational inequality: A sociological perspective. J Econ Perspect. 2002;16: 31–44. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 78. Galobardes B. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). J Epidemiol Community Heal. 2006;60: 7–12. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.023531 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 79. Erikson R. Social Class of Men, Women and Families. Sociology. 1984;18: 500–514. doi: 10.1177/0038038584018004003 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 80. Dennison CR. Intergenerational Mobility and Changes in Drug Use Across the Life Course. J Drug Issues. 2018;48: 205–225. doi: 10.1177/0022042617746974 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 81. Ueno K, Peña-Talamantes AE, Roach TA. Sexual Orientation and Occupational Attainment. Work Occup. 2013;40: 3–36. doi: 10.1177/0730888412460532 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 82. Nam CB, Boyd M. Occupational Status in 2000; Over a Century of Census-Based Measurement. Popul Res Policy Rev. 2004;23: 327–358. doi: 10.1023/B:POPU.0000040045.51228.34 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 83. Braveman P, Egerter S, Williams DR. The Social Determinants of Health: Coming of Age. Annu Rev Public Health. 2011;32: 381–398. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031210-101218 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 84. Surachman A, Rice C, Bray B, Gruenewald T, Almeida D. Association Between Socioeconomic Status Mobility and Inflammation Markers Among White and Black Adults in the United States: A Latent Class Analysis. Psychosom Med. 2020;82. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000752 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 85. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1: 385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 86. Kaiser C. DRM: Stata module to fit Sobel’s Diagonal Reference Model (DRM). 2018.
  • 87. Jaime-Castillo AM, Marqués-Perales I. Social mobility and demand for redistribution in Europe: A comparative analysis. Br J Sociol. 2019;70: 138–165. doi: 10.1111/1468-4446.12363 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 88. Gugushvili A, Zhao Y, Bukodi E. Intergenerational educational mobility and smoking: a study of 20 European countries using diagonal reference models. Public Health. 2020;181: 94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2019.12.009 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 89. Paskov M, Präg P, Richards L. Does downward social mobility make people more hostile towards immigrants? Oxford; 2019. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 90. James SA. John Henryism, Structural Racism, and Cardiovascular Health Risks in Black Americans. Racism: Science & Tools for the Public Health Professional. 2019. doi: 10.2105/9780875533049ch08 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 91. Zhao Y, Li Y, Heath A, Shryane N. Inter- and intra-generational social mobility effects on subjective well-being—Evidence from mainland China. Res Soc Stratif Mobil. 2017;48: 54–66. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2017.02.002 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 92. Chan TW. Social mobility and the well-being of individuals. Br J Sociol. 2018;69: 183–206. doi: 10.1111/1468-4446.12285 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Decision Letter 0

Jennifer morozink boylan.

31 Mar 2021

PONE-D-21-03406

Socioeconomic Position, Social Mobility, and Health Selection Effects on Allostatic Load In the United States

Dear Dr. Gugushvili,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I have read your manuscript and received input from three expert reviewers. All reviewers note key strengths of the study but also raise important concerns. Reviewers 2 and 3 both note how findings reported here on race differences in the associations between social mobility and allostatic load differ from previously published findings in the same cohort. In the revision, please include discussion of this discrepancy and likewise respond to comments from Reviewer 2 regarding statistical power to test interactions by race and how Hispanic ethnicity was treated. Each reviewer requested additional methodological details, and the authors are especially encouraged to provide more information regarding the operationalization of social mobility, conceptualization of allostatic load, and the interpretation of coefficients from diagonal reference models. Finally, please ensure that the study is reported in accordance with guidelines from STROBE ( https://www.strobe-statement.org/ ), in particular that the study design is included in the title or abstract and that limitations of observational research are acknowledged, including in the Abstract.

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 15 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at [email protected] . When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see:  http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols . Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at  https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols .

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Academic Editor

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. PLOS requires an ORCID iD for the corresponding author in Editorial Manager on papers submitted after December 6th, 2016. Please ensure that you have an ORCID iD and that it is validated in Editorial Manager. To do this, go to ‘Update my Information’ (in the upper left-hand corner of the main menu), and click on the Fetch/Validate link next to the ORCID field. This will take you to the ORCID site and allow you to create a new iD or authenticate a pre-existing iD in Editorial Manager. Please see the following video for instructions on linking an ORCID iD to your Editorial Manager account: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xcclfuvtxQ

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information .

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #3: I Don't Know

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper examined the effect of social mobility on AL using the data from Add Health Study. The authors found that short-range upward mobility had a positive impact on health, and such a health benefit gain associated with social upward mobility was only observed for participants reporting poor health during adolescence. Overall, the strength of this paper is the inclusion of a large, national sample. However, there were also a couple issues of this current form. My major concern is that the lack of AL data at baseline may not able to completely differentiate between health selection and social causation effects in this study, thought the authors included self-reported health in the model. However, self-reported health may not always reflect the objective health status. The variances of AL that was explained by self-reported health was relatively low. Another concern is that participants were relatively young and that their social standings relative to their patients may more likely to be subjective to change. That is the downward social mobility observed in this study may likely to be temporary, and that is could be one possible explanation that this study observed a null effect of downward mobility on AL. Other issues were detailed below.

1. A recent article examined the effects of social mobility on inflammation may be helpful to discuss the introduction of social mobility and health.

Surachman, A., Rice, C., Bray, B., Gruenewald, T., & Almeida, D. (2020). Association between socioeconomic status mobility and inflammation markers among White and black adults in the United States: a latent class analysis. Psychosomatic medicine, 82(2), 224-233.

2. It is unclear how short-range and long-range were operationalized. To what extent of the changes in SES was referred to as one-step mobility?

3. It is unclear how self-reported heath was assessed. Was it assessed using one or multiple items? What were the response options?

4. Medical chronic health conditions may affect both educational attainment and occupation, as well as AL. It would be interesting to know if results reported to this study would be robust after controlling for medical chronic health conditions.

5. In Table 2, please specify how low and high SES quintiles were operationalized.

6. The author stated in the limitation section that the upward or downward social mobility observed in this study may change and this is may be particular for occupational attainment. I would like to know if the results observed this study would be similar when SES is assessed using educational attainment as the single indictor. This may help to shed some light on this issue.

Reviewer #2: This manuscript investigates the influence of childhood and adult socioeconomic status on health using public data from Add Health and diagonal reference models. The authors find that short range upward mobility is associated with improved health, while long range upward mobility and downward mobility are not significantly associated with health. They find no evidence of heterogeneity by sociodemographic characteristics.

I have two major comments. First, the authors construct a measure of health from biomarker data. They call this measure allostatic load. Allostatic load, as the authors state as well (203), is a measure of neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic and cardiovascular function. The authors only use measures of metabolic and cardiovascular function (as well as one inflammatory measure). They would be better served calling this a measure of cardiometabolic health, rather than allostatic load. More generally, there is a need to more fully introduce and motivate the measure of health that is used and why it is appropriate for the given analysis. The authors also mention that self-rated health is limited and allostatic load addresses these limitations, but then rely on self-rated health to account for health selection. Why not use a measure of BMI or childhood illnesses to test for health selection? Or temper the discussion of self-rated health. Moreover, in their discussion the authors state (442): “The main explanation for this could be that our AL index based on neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic, and cardiovascular system functioning, is more sensitive to lifetime exposures and experiences, while alternative measures such as, for instance, health-related behaviours and perceptions are more likely to be shaped by individuals’ contemporary conditions.” This is an incorrect description of the measure of AL that they were able to construct.

Second, the authors conduct tests of moderation in the relationship between SES and health by race. This analysis is important, but the authors do not appropriately motivate the analysis. There is a large literature that is not cited, including foundational work by Sherman James on John Henryism, and more recent work on skin-deep resilience by Brody, Miller and Chen. Moreover, the racial categories constructed are unusual for the US, where Hispanic ethnicity is usually accounted for in some way. Are the Black and White groups including Hispanic ethnicity here? I’d suggest the authors also read this recent article on how to discuss results on racial disparities in health: https://www.healthaffairs.org/do/10.1377/hblog20200630.939347/full/?utm_medium=social&utm_sour=& . Note the language used on page 452 – talking about the health effects of race. I also wonder whether, given the use of the public data in Add Health and the limited sample size, if the authors are adequately powered to estimate these interactions. The findings are much less conclusive if this is the case.

A few minor points:

- Wave V of Add Health is now available. Why not use? This would alleviate concern over early adult SES.

- Line 176 – testing if position and mobility effects differ by socioeconomic groups – what are the socioeconomic groups? Do they mean sociodemographic characteristics of race, gender, etc?

- Settlement type is a strange way to refer to urban/rural in a US context.

- Line 457 – Mentions psychological benefits, but no tests of this in the analysis – goes beyond the data in discussion

- Some references from Add Health that are relevant but not mentioned:

o Brody, Gene H.; Yu, Tianyi; Miller, Gregory E.; & Chen, Edith (2016). Resilience in adolescence, health, and psychosocial outcomes. Pediatrics, 138(6), e20161042. PMCID: PMC5127063

o Chen, Edith; Yu, Tianyi; Siliezar, Rebekah; Drage, Jane N.; Dezil, Johanna; Miller, Gregory E.; & Brody, Gene H. (2020). Evidence for skin-deep resilience using a co-twin control design: Effects on low-grade inflammation in a longitudinal study of youth. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. PMCID: PMC7415558

o Miller, Gregory E.; Chen, Edith; Yu, Tianyi; & Brody, Gene H. (2020). Youth Who Achieve Upward Socioeconomic Mobility Display Lower Psychological Distress But Higher Metabolic Syndrome Rates as Adults: Prospective Evidence From Add Health and MIDUS. Journal of the American Heart Association, 9(9). PMCID: PMC7428555

o Gaydosh, Lauren; Schorpp, Kristen M.; Chen, Edith; Miller, Gregory E.; & Harris, Kathleen Mullan (2018). College completion predicts lower depression but higher metabolic syndrome among disadvantaged minorities in young adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(1), 109-114. PMCID: PMC5776811

o Belsky, Daniel W.; Domingue, Benjamin W.; Wedow, Robbee; Arseneault, Louise; Boardman, Jason D.; Caspi, Avshalom; Conley, Dalton; Fletcher, Jason M.; Freese, Jeremy; & Herd, Pamela, et al. (2018). Genetic analysis of social-class mobility in five longitudinal studies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

o Yang, Yang Claire; Gerken, Karen; Schorpp, Kristen M.; Boen, Courtney; & Harris, Kathleen Mullan (2017). Early-Life Socioeconomic Status and Adult Physiological Functioning: A Life Course Examination of Biosocial Mechanisms. Biodemography and Social Biology, 63(2), 87-103. PMCID: PMC5439296

Reviewer #3: Manuscript PONE-D-21-03406 “Socioeconomic position, social mobility, and health selection effects on allostatic load in the United States” explores associations between socioeconomic conditions, social mobility, and indicators of physical health (i.e. allostatic load) in a large an well-studied sample of US adults, the Add Health sample, who have been followed from adolescence to young adulthood. Authors observed a negative association between socioeconomic position and allostatic load. They also observed that “origin” and “destination” socioeconomic circumstances were similarly associated with allostatic load and found an association between short-range upward social mobility and lower allostatic load, particularly among individuals who self-reported worse health at the initial wave of Add Health. The manuscript is well written, comprehensive, and the findings are compelling. The findings will be of interest to those concerned with associations between socioeconomic circumstances and health. I only have a few comments.

1. I am not an expert in diagonal reference models (DRMs), so most of my comments concern this method. My feeling after reading the introduction and methods is that much was said about DRMs (pg 4, pg 12-13), but that still more could be said (perhaps even in simpler and more applied terms) to make the unfamiliar reader better understand the rationale for (and interpretation of) DRMs to address questions about social mobility. One applied example provided by the authors is: “These associations could be, at least partially, explained by the fact that the upwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the bottom quintile, while downwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the highest quintile.” (pg 14), but I found this logic confusing because it seems that, by definition, upwardly mobile groups cannot end up in the bottom quintile. So, the less familiar reader is still not clear on what exactly DRMs are and how they can address limitations in the prior literature.

2. For readers less familiar with DRM methods, it would be helpful if there was clearer interpretation about what exactly each type of parameter is indicating in Table 3. For example, in the full model (Model 5), what is the exact interpretation of the coefficient for the intercept? The interpretation of the estimate for “short-range upward” is straightforward enough, given the earlier explanation that “Social mobility variables and associated point estimates,…, in DRM approach could be interpreted in the same way as in a conventional regression model, a reference category being a group of individuals with the same socioeconomic position as their parents.” (pg 13). In Model 5 of Table 3, the point estimate for “Lowest” is 0.17. Is its p-value below 0.001 indicating that this AL estimate of 0.17 is significantly greater than 0 (the mean of AL in the sample)? Similarly, when interpreting the weight parameters in Model 1, authors state that “The calculated weight parameters in Model 1 shows that the relative importance of parental socioeconomic position (0.35, CI 0.19,0.51) is lower than the importance of individuals’ own socioeconomic position (0.65, CI 0.49,0.81).” (pg 14). What is the exact interpretation of a weight of 0.65?

3. In Models 2 and 3 of Table 3, with the addition of the social mobility variables, the “importance” of own socioeconomic position seems to drop far below the importance of parental socioeconomic position. This seems counterintuitive given that there is, simultaneously, a statistically significant association between upward social mobility and AL. Similarly, in Models 4 and 5 of Table 3, Origin and destination weights seem approximately similar, but still, destination “importance” seems smaller than origin. Given that a significant association was found for short-range upward mobility, I would have guessed that destination would have had a slightly stronger weight. Some interpretation of these specific findings would be helpful.

4. In Table 4, authors observed no interaction between race and mobility on AL. This finding would seem to contrast with the findings of Gaydosh et al., 2017, “College completion predicts lower depression but higher metabolic syndrome among disadvantaged minorities in young adulthood”, Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences, 115(1), 109-114, who, also utilizing Add Health data, observed some physical health costs associated with upward social mobility among Black and Hispanic adults. Of course, the methods utilized in the two studies are different, but interpretation of findings from the current study considering these previous findings in Add Health are needed.

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article ( what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy .

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool,  https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/ . PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at  [email protected] . Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Author response to Decision Letter 0

Collection date 2021.

18 May 2021

Dear Editor and Reviewers,

Thank you very much for your reviews and giving us the opportunity to revise our manuscript.

We have now completed all suggested and required revisions. We believe that the review process has improved the quality of our study. Below we provide the detailed explanations how we have dealt with all points raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for your attention on this matter.

Best wishes

Authors of the manuscript (PONE-D-21-03406)

I have read your manuscript and received input from three expert reviewers. All reviewers note key strengths of the study but also raise important concerns.

### Thank you very much for your work on our submission.

Reviewers 2 and 3 both note how findings reported here on race differences in the associations between social mobility and allostatic load differ from previously published findings in the same cohort. In the revision, please include discussion of this discrepancy and likewise respond to comments from Reviewer 2 regarding statistical power to test interactions by race and how Hispanic ethnicity was treated.

### We now explicitly address the discrepancy between our findings and those reported by Gaydosh et al. Please find our detailed response and explanation after the last comment of Reviewer 3 and see as well the changes in the main text. To briefly summarise here, there are three main methodological reasons (1. different models, 2. different health outcomes, and 3. different samples) which are likely to explain the observed difference. As for statistical power to test interactions, we now explicitly state that our findings should be interpreted with caution as for the selected interactions the sample size may be too small to provide sufficient variation and conclusive findings.

Each reviewer requested additional methodological details, and the authors are especially encouraged to provide more information regarding the operationalization of social mobility, conceptualization of allostatic load, and the interpretation of coefficients from diagonal reference models.

### As you can see below in our answers to reviewers’ comments, we now provide more information in Research Design section on operationalization of social mobility, conceptualization of allostatic load, and the interpretation of coefficients from diagonal reference models.

Finally, please ensure that the study is reported in accordance with guidelines from STROBE ( https://www.strobe-statement.org/ ), in particular that the study design is included in the title or abstract and that limitations of observational research are acknowledged, including in the Abstract.

### Now abstract clearly states that we use data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health’s Waves I and IV and that we are able only to mitigate (not eliminate) health selection concerns in observational data we use.

REVIEWER #1:

This paper examined the effect of social mobility on AL using the data from Add Health Study. The authors found that short-range upward mobility had a positive impact on health, and such a health benefit gain associated with social upward mobility was only observed for participants reporting poor health during adolescence. Overall, the strength of this paper is the inclusion of a large, national sample. However, there were also a couple issues of this current form.

### Thank you for this assessment.

My major concern is that the lack of AL data at baseline may not able to completely differentiate between health selection and social causation effects in this study, thought the authors included self-reported health in the model. However, self-reported health may not always reflect the objective health status. The variances of AL that was explained by self-reported health was relatively low.

### Thank you for pointing this out. The survey does not include AL in Wave I, yet we address your concern on accounting the baseline health by additionally controlling for BMI and chronic health problems (including heart problems, asthma, diabetes and difficulties using limbs) at Wave I. The results are shown now in the updated Table 5 on page 21. Accounting for these additional variables for initial health did not affect our main results.

Another concern is that participants were relatively young and that their social standings relative to their patients may more likely to be subjective to change. That is the downward social mobility observed in this study may likely to be temporary, and that is could be one possible explanation that this study observed a null effect of downward mobility on AL. Other issues were detailed below.

### Thank you for this note. We completely agree that downward mobility experiences might be followed by upward mobility experiences and this is can be an interesting topic to explore in another study. However, we also argue that health and wellbeing outcomes relating to social mobility among individuals being in their late 20s are a socially important topic to investigate. Still, based on your suggestion, we now include the following note on page 23 in the discussion section:

“It is important to note that due to relatively young age composition of the analytical sample, for many individuals the downward social mobility observed in this study may be temporary, which could be one possible explanation why we find a null effect of downward mobility on AL."

The issue of downward mobility being only temporary is further examined by studding health outcomes for educational mobility separately in supplementary materials (Table S6). In the case of educational mobility, it can be plausibly assumed that most of the respondents achieved their lifetime attainment. We do not find that downward or upward educational mobility is associated with worse or better health outcomes.

### Thank you for this. We now cite this reference in the paper, see lines 285-288 on page 12.

### We explain this now in more details, please see the text in the third paragraph on page 11:

“Finally, to derive the index of socioeconomic position for parents and individuals, we combined educational and occupational attainment variables. This resulted in scores ranging from 2 to 10 points for the highest achieving individuals. To ensure that each mobility group had adequate representation, we collapsed the combined socioeconomic position scores into quintiles, where quintile 5 represents the top 20% (highest attainment based on educational and occupational status). From these combined measures we calculated intergenerational social mobility variables. We subtracted parental from individuals scores. This resulted in a mobility measure ranging from -4 to 4, where 0 represents the immobile group. For example, if the respondent achieved a score equal 5, highest attainment (top quintile) and parental attainment was equal to 4, the difference between the two scores produces one-step upward mobility.

To ensure sufficient variation we collapse two, three and four steps into a long-range mobility indicator, separately for upward and downward mobility, while one-step mobility represents short-range mobility.”

### We now provide a specific wording of the question and answers on self-rated health question on page 12, lines 291-293:

“More specifically, the respondents were asked the following question: “In general, how is your health?” which they could rate on from 1 (= poor) to 5 (= excellent) health.”

### Table 5 in the main results presents findings when Wave 1 chronic health as well as BMI scores are accounted for. This new initial health variables do not significantly change the main results.

### We now describe how SEP quintiles were operationalised in greater detail in the main text of the paper on page 11, paragraph 4. More specifically we write that:

“…to derive the index of socioeconomic position for parents and individuals, we combined educational and occupational attainment variables. This resulted in scores ranging from 2 to 10 points for the highest achieving individuals. To ensure that each mobility group had adequate representation, we collapsed the combined socioeconomic position scores into quintiles, where quintile 5 represents the top 20% (highest attainment based on educational and occupational status). From these combined measures we calculated intergenerational social mobility variables.”

Lines 350-16. The author stated in the limitation section that the upward or downward social mobility observed in this study may change and this is may be particular for occupational attainment. I would like to know if the results observed this study would be similar when SES is assessed using educational attainment as the single indictor. This may help to shed some light on this issue.

### Based on your suggestion, we now introduce additional analysis in supplementary materials in which we only account for mobility in educational attainment. The following text appears on page 21, lines 458-462:

“It is possible that the respondents are still too young to be certain that downward mobility will not change as time passes. This may be particularly true in terms of occupational attainment. To address this issue, in supplementary materials, Table S6, we estimate our models with education as the only SEP measure and educational mobility parameters. These results show no educational mobility effects, while in terms of health gradient and the importance of the relative weight, no major differences were observed.”

REVIEWER #2:

This manuscript investigates the influence of childhood and adult socioeconomic status on health using public data from Add Health and diagonal reference models. The authors find that short range upward mobility is associated with improved health, while long range upward mobility and downward mobility are not significantly associated with health. They find no evidence of heterogeneity by sociodemographic characteristics.

### Thank you. Yes, indeed this largely correct assessment of our study.

I have two major comments. First, the authors construct a measure of health from biomarker data. They call this measure allostatic load. Allostatic load, as the authors state as well (203), is a measure of neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic and cardiovascular function. The authors only use measures of metabolic and cardiovascular function (as well as one inflammatory measure). They would be better served calling this a measure of cardiometabolic health, rather than allostatic load.

### Thank you for this comment. We changed the description of AL measure and added additional motivation lines. On page 9, we now first state that AL index may incorporate

“… neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic, and cardiovascular system functioning and is a validated predictor of morbidity and mortality outcomes, especially at the earlier stages of life.”

In other words, this sentence implies that AL does not have to incorporate all listed areas of bodily functions. Following this clarification on the same page, lines 220-223, we state that:

“Our approach to constructing this measure is closely matched with the previous research in which AL is based on lipid and glucose metabolism, inflammation (C-reactive protein and fibrinogen), body fat deposition (body mass index and waist measurement) and cardiovascular measures [30].”

However, we are happy to substitute the name of our outcome variable to ‘cardiometabolic function’ if the editor thinks we should do so.

More generally, there is a need to more fully introduce and motivate the measure of health that is used and why it is appropriate for the given analysis. The authors also mention that self-rated health is limited and allostatic load addresses these limitations, but then rely on self-rated health to account for health selection. Why not use a measure of BMI or childhood illnesses to test for health selection? Or temper the discussion of self-rated health. Moreover, in their discussion the authors state (442): “The main explanation for this could be that our AL index based on neuroendocrine, immune, metabolic, and cardiovascular system functioning, is more sensitive to lifetime exposures and experiences, while alternative measures such as, for instance, health-related behaviours and perceptions are more likely to be shaped by individuals’ contemporary conditions.” This is an incorrect description of the measure of AL that they were able to construct.

### Thank you for this comment. We have now changed the description of AL measure and added additional motivation in the first and the first paragraph on page 10. We note that:

“…we consider this measure to be particularly appropriate for our study as it is able to capture even relatively small changes in young adults’ health. This is especially important in the context of social mobility where the sensitivity of health outcome measures has yielded in mixed results.”

As for using alternative measures of health selection, we note that self-rated health has stronger association with AL than other indicators. This measure also gives us an opportunity to divide the sample into those reporting better and worse health. Nonetheless, based on your suggestion, in the main analysis, Table 5, we now present results with Wave 1 BMI and chronic health problems are accounted for.

Second, the authors conduct tests of moderation in the relationship between SES and health by race. This analysis is important, but the authors do not appropriately motivate the analysis. There is a large literature that is not cited, including foundational work by Sherman James on John Henryism, and more recent work on skin-deep resilience by Brody, Miller and Chen.

### We have now refer and cite the suggested sources on, among other areas, structural racism, the disproportionate stressors experienced by racialized groups. Please see the second paragraph on page 7, and we also state in the main text on page 19:

“Past research on various health outcomes in the United States indicates that due to historical and structural factors, including discrimination, racial/ethnic differences in social mobility’s effect on AL may be present [81,87].”

Moreover, the racial categories constructed are unusual for the US, where Hispanic ethnicity is usually accounted for in some way. Are the Black and White groups including Hispanic ethnicity here? I’d suggest the authors also read this recent article on how to discuss results on racial disparities in health: https://www.healthaffairs.org/do/10.1377/hblog20200630.939347/full/?utm_medium=social&utm_sour=& . Note the language used on page 452 – talking about the health effects of race.

### Thank you for this note and suggestion. We now account for Hispanic ethnicity in the updated race/ethnicity variable which includes the following four categories: Whites (60% of the analytical sample), Blacks (24%), Hispanics (11%) and other category (5%). This operationalisation is similar what Gaydosh et al. use in their study (College completion predicts lower 726 depression but higher metabolic syndrome among disadvantaged minorities in young adulthood. 727 Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: 109–114). All results are now updated using this new race/ethnicity variable but this does not affect our original findings.

I also wonder whether, given the use of the public data in Add Health and the limited sample size, if the authors are adequately powered to estimate these interactions. The findings are much less conclusive if this is the case.

### On pages 19-20, lines 435-437, as suggested, we added a point about caution in interpreting these interactions. We state that…

“It should be emphasised that these findings, mostly insignificant, should be interpreted with caution as for the selected interactions the sample size may be too small to provide sufficient variation and conclusive findings.”

### We started working on this paper quite some time before public version of Wave V became available. Adjusting our analytical sample and the age composition would likely change the framing and results of the study, yet in the discussion section we explicitly state that data from Wave V would be very helpful to understand the effects of social mobility on later health outcomes among Add Health participants.

### Yes, indeed, we meant to say socioeconomic groups. The following edited text now appears in the end of the introduction section, page 8, lines 181-182:

“…testing if position and mobility effects differ by sociodemographic characteristics such as gender and race;”

### We have now replaced “settlement type” with urban/rural divide throughout the text.

### Thank you for pointing this out. Yes, we are mentioning the possible psychological benefits of upward mobility, but we do this primarily to describe potential mechanisms linking social mobility and health. In this study, we did not intend to test physiological effects of mobility as there are other studies on this topic.

### At the start of “2.2. Measures” section, we now include the suggested references by noting that:

“...Numerous past studies used the Add Health to investigate the impact of socioeconomic position on physical [60–62] and mental [47,53,63] health outcomes.”

REVIEWER #3:

Manuscript PONE-D-21-03406 “Socioeconomic position, social mobility, and health selection effects on allostatic load in the United States” explores associations between socioeconomic conditions, social mobility, and indicators of physical health (i.e. allostatic load) in a large an well-studied sample of US adults, the Add Health sample, who have been followed from adolescence to young adulthood. Authors observed a negative association between socioeconomic position and allostatic load. They also observed that “origin” and “destination” socioeconomic circumstances were similarly associated with allostatic load and found an association between short-range upward social mobility and lower allostatic load, particularly among individuals who self-reported worse health at the initial wave of Add Health. The manuscript is well written, comprehensive, and the findings are compelling. The findings will be of interest to those concerned with associations between socioeconomic circumstances and health. I only have a few comments.

### Thank you very much for this kind assessment.

### Thank you for this observation. Because we did not intend this paper to be methodologically oriented, we do not provide a comprehensive description of DRM approach. However, it is largely accepted in the literature on the consequences of social mobility that the DRM approach is superior to alternatives and we refer readers to some of the recent studies on this topic. Nonetheless, based on your suggestion, we introduce further details in the description of DRM by emphasizing on pages 13-14 that:

(1) “conventional statistical models cannot simultaneously include origin, destination, and mobility parameters;”

(2) “off-diagonal cells in two-dimensional table represent specific mobility trajectories;” and

(3) “an extensive overview of this statistical method, its usefulness in modelling of social mobility effects, and a comparison with conventional regression approaches are described and demonstrated elsewhere”.

We also add to this section with the following message at the bottom of page 15:

“For various empirical applications of DRM approach in different countries and contexts readers can refer to studies on, among other areas, redistribution preferences [84], likelihood of smoking [85], attitudes toward immigrants [86].

As for the statement given on page 14 of the original submission (“These associations could be, at least partially, explained by the fact that the upwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the bottom quintile, while downwardly mobile groups do not include those who ended up in the highest quintile”), we modified it and added further information in the following sentence, lines 362-365, on page 15:

“The latter also suggests that upwardly and downwardly mobile individuals differ by their social origin and destination positions and to disentangle these position effects from social mobility effects, we employed the above-described statistical approach – DRM.”

2. For readers less familiar with DRM methods, it would be helpful if there was clearer interpretation about what exactly each type of parameter is indicating in Table 3. For example, in the full model (Model 5), what is the exact interpretation of the coefficient for the intercept?

### Thank you for this clarifying question. As we mentioned earlier in the text, the parameters of DRM models should be interpreted as ordinary regression models, which means that the reported intercept show mean AL for individuals with all covariates being equal to zero. However, considering that the reported coefficients for immobile individuals in respective socioeconomic positions are essentially class-specific intercepts, we decided to remove the global intercept from the models as it does not help in understanding of the models, does not have a substantive meaning, and some of the most relevant studies using DRM approach also do not report global intercepts. See for instance the following articles:

Houle, Jason N. 2011. “The Psychological Impact of Intragenerational Social Class Mobility.” Social Science Research 40(3):757–72.

van der Waal, Jeroen, Stijn Daenekindt, and Willem de Koster. 2017. “Statistical Challenges in Modelling the Health Consequences of Social Mobility: The Need for Diagonal Reference Models.” International Journal of Public Health 62(9):1029–37.

The interpretation of the estimate for “short-range upward” is straightforward enough, given the earlier explanation that “Social mobility variables and associated point estimates,…, in DRM approach could be interpreted in the same way as in a conventional regression model, a reference category being a group of individuals with the same socioeconomic position as their parents.” (pg 13). In Model 5 of Table 3, the point estimate for “Lowest” is 0.17. Is its p-value below 0.001 indicating that this AL estimate of 0.17 is significantly greater than 0 (the mean of AL in the sample)?

### Thank you for this clarifying question. Class coefficients indicate the class-specific intercepts. In other words, the coefficients represent weighted mean values of AL for those who occupy diagonal cells in our two-dimensional five by five matrix. We added more clarifying information on this matter in the first paragraph of Section 3.2 on page 16.

Similarly, when interpreting the weight parameters in Model 1, authors state that “The calculated weight parameters in Model 1 shows that the relative importance of parental socioeconomic position (0.35, CI 0.19,0.51) is lower than the importance of individuals’ own socioeconomic position (0.65, CI 0.49,0.81).” (pg 14). What is the exact interpretation of a weight of 0.65?

### Thank you for this comment. Weight greater than 0.5 indicates greater importance of destination characteristics (position) in determining AL. The destination weight (w) ranges between 0 and 1, with 0 indicating that the destination class plays no role for determining current AL and 1 indicating that it is only the destination that explains the variation in AL. The origin weight equals to 1-w. Now, after editing the corresponding section, you can find the following text on page 16, the second paragraph:

“The calculated weight parameters in Model 1 show that the relative importance of parental socioeconomic position (0.35, CI 0.19,0.51) is lower than the importance of individuals’ own socioeconomic position (0.65, CI 0.49,0.81), which means that almost twice as much variation in the outcome variable is explained by individuals’ destination than by their origin.”

### In a model, where no social mobility parameters are included, the coefficients for origin and destination capture the corresponding direct and indirect effects via mobility. When the mobility effects are captured, the relative importance of origin characteristics changes as it then only accounts for direct origin effects. Similarly, we may look at the destination’s importance and observe that its relative importance decreases once mobility effects are accounted for. This change of the relative weight importance is common in past research. For instance, see two recent studies published in Social Science and Medicine:

Gugushvili, Alexi, Yizhang Zhao, and Erzsébet Bukodi. 2019. “‘Falling from Grace’ and ‘Rising from Rags’: Intergenerational Educational Mobility and Depressive Symptoms.” Social Science & Medicine 222:294–304.

Steiber, Nadia. 2019. “Intergenerational Educational Mobility and Health Satisfaction across the Life Course: Does the Long Arm of Childhood Conditions Only Become Visible Later in Life?” Social Science & Medicine 242:112603.

### Thank you very much for this point. We now explicitly refer to this study in the discussion section when describing the main findings of our study. In the second paragraph on page 24, you can find the following text:

“Our finding contrast with results from past research using the same dataset, suggesting that selected minority groups (Blacks and Hispanics) experience higher metabolic syndrome after college completion [56]. Three main methodological aspects are likely to explain this difference. First, our research strategy is focused on disentangling mobility effect from origin and destination effects, based on SEP derived from educational and occupational attainment, and using DRM approach, while Gaydosh et al. rely only on educational mobility and use conventional Poisson regressions. Second, health measures also differ noticeably as Gaydosh et al. rely on metabolic syndrome based on blood pressure, glycosylated hemoglobin, body to waist ratio and cholesterol, while AL measure used in our study in addition to components such as blood pressure and cholesterol includes other biomarkers including CRP and BMI. Third, Gaydosh et al. use the restricted-full sample, while we use the public version of Add Health. These differences make any direct comparison across the two studies difficult.”

Submitted filename: R&R_PLOSONE_Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

28 Jun 2021

Socioeconomic Position, Social Mobility, and Health Selection Effects on Allostatic Load in the United States

PONE-D-21-03406R1

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ , click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at [email protected] .

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact [email protected] .

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

I support the authors regarding their preference to refer to their outcome as allostatic load.

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

6. Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: The authors have done a good job addressing the critiques raised by myself and the other two reviewers. I have no further major comments

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article ( what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

Acceptance letter

14 Jul 2021

Dear Dr. Gugushvili:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact [email protected] .

If we can help with anything else, please email us at [email protected] .

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Jennifer Morozink Boylan

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data availability statement.

  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (665.2 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

IMAGES

  1. ⇉Social Mobility Definition Essay Example

    thesis on social mobility

  2. (PDF) Essays on Social Mobility and Distribution

    thesis on social mobility

  3. 📚 Essay Example on Social Mobility Research

    thesis on social mobility

  4. Social Mobility: The Dissociative Thesis and its Disputants

    thesis on social mobility

  5. Social mobility

    thesis on social mobility

  6. HR Magazine

    thesis on social mobility

VIDEO

  1. Arish Alreja

  2. 2024 maintaining consistency in your thesis

  3. Writing your Master's thesis? Check this out!

  4. PRE-ARCHITECTURE THESIS: NYANZA SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT CENTER

  5. Dirty Paraffin rocks the Thesis Social Jam in Soweto!

  6. My 3mn PhD Thesis UNITA

COMMENTS

  1. PDF Essays on Social Mobility

    This thesis comprises three empirical studies that explore contemporary topics within the area of social mobility. I investigate the mechanisms that contribute to the perpetuation of inequalities in the UK. In the first empirical chapter, I examine the degree of intergenerational income mobility in the UK.

  2. PDF Understanding the Nature and Consequences of Social Mobility Beliefs

    That is, people may focus on expected social mobility, which has yet to occur; expe-rienced social mobility, which has already occurred; or both (i.e., beliefs about social class change from some point in the past to some point in the future). Although experienced social mobility reasonably would inform expected social mobility

  3. The Experience of Social Mobility: Social Isolation, Utilitarian

    The dissociative thesis states that social mobility is a disruptive and detrimental experience for the individual. Despite the absence of convincing evidence either for or against it, this thesis is generally accepted in sociology. I investigate this thesis by considering three dimensions of dissociation—i.e., social isolation, utilitarian individualism, and social disorientation. I use data ...

  4. Social mobility: Concepts, theories and hypotheses

    For example, the LZ thesis has been superseded by the NH hypothesis, and basic percentages have been replaced by odds ratios. ... Further, it seems that social mobility is a topic which encourages ...

  5. PDF Personal Social Mobility Beliefs and Motivation for Goals

    A Thesis submitted to the School of Graduate Studies in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science ... Social mobility beliefs refer to the perceived likelihood of status change occurring over time. There are two typical ways to measure social mobility. It can be assessed over people's

  6. PDF An Analysis of Social Mobility in Contemporary Shanghai

    mobility((Moro-Egido, Navarro & Sánchez, 2022; Becker & Birkelbach, 2018). Social mobility and equality of opportunity can be measured from earnings, income, or social class, but can also be understood to encompass other well-being dimensions such as health and education. Firstly, intergenerational mobility means the social mobility of individuals

  7. PDF The Experience of Social Mobility: Social Isolation, Utilitarian

    experience of social mobility and to reassess its potentially detrimental consequences for the individual. 2 Theory 2.1 The Dissociative Thesis Intergenerational social mobility refers to the situation where individuals have a different position in the social hierarchy—be it higher or lower—than their parents. Habits, atti-

  8. Intergenerational class mobility and the convergence thesis

    The extraordinary trio of papers by Robert Erikson, John H. Goldthorpe and Lucienne Portocarero in the British Journal of Sociology in 1979, 1982, and 1983 advanced thinking and research in the field of social stratification and mobility. Their agenda became the agenda of the whole research committee on stratification and mobility within the International Sociological Association (known as ...

  9. Essays on Social Mobility

    Essays on Social Mobility Rohenkohl, Bertha ORCID: 0000-0002-1538-7039 (2020) Essays on Social Mobility. PhD thesis, University of Sheffield. Metadata. Supervisors: Dickerson, Andy and Ratcliffe, Anita and Van de Sijpe, Nicolas: Keywords:

  10. Socioeconomic position, social mobility, and health selection effects

    Sorokin's dissociative thesis views upward social mobility as a deviation from expected continuity associated with individuals' social origins . Adjusting to an unfamiliar socioeconomic environment, while also socially distancing from the familiar and more natural past environment, can be a major stress-inducing process compromising ...